In Milgram’s most famous experiment (Milgram, 1963), two
participants entered a room and drew lots to be assigned either the role of
‘Teacher’ or ‘Learner’. The Teacher’s role was to help the Learner memorize
word pairs by administering an electric shock for wrong answers, the voltage
increasing for each subsequent incorrect response. The Teacher helped strap the
Learner into an electric chair, then began the procedure from the next room over
a two-way intercom. In actuality, the
lot had been rigged and the confederate Learner received no electric shocks,
which is fortunate considering 98% of Teachers ‘shocked’ the Learner with the
maximum 450 volts (labelled ‘warning: extreme shock’ for good measure) despite
groans, screams of pain, begging to be released, and finally a chilling silence
from the next room. The only motivation the Teacher needed to continue torturing
an innocent man was an order from a position of authority: the experimenter.
With these disturbing findings in mind, Milgram (1965) carried out several further experiments investigating how varying the Teacher’s interaction with the Learner influenced levels of obedience. In the first condition, the victim could not be seen or heard, except for pounding on the wall at 300 volts which stopped immediately after 315 volts had been administered (Remote Feedback). The second condition had the victim protesting loudly as in the classic experiment, which could be heard through a slightly ajar door and reverberating through the walls of the laboratory (Voice Feedback). A third condition had the victim in the same room as the Teacher about 1½ feet away, so his screams of pain could be heard, as well as be seen writhing in his chair (Proximity). The fourth condition had the victim receive the shock through an electrified plate; meaning beyond 150 volts the Teacher had to force down the Learner’s resisting hand to administer further punishment (Touch-Proximity).
Unsurprisingly, obedience dropped the more contact the Teacher had with the Learner: 34% defiance in the Remote condition, 37.5% with Voice Feedback, 60% in Proximity, and 70% in Touch-Proximity. The original graph is displayed below. Despite this drop in obedience, it should still be noted a worrying 30% of subjects would still physically force a protesting innocent to receive a potentially fatal electric shock.
With these disturbing findings in mind, Milgram (1965) carried out several further experiments investigating how varying the Teacher’s interaction with the Learner influenced levels of obedience. In the first condition, the victim could not be seen or heard, except for pounding on the wall at 300 volts which stopped immediately after 315 volts had been administered (Remote Feedback). The second condition had the victim protesting loudly as in the classic experiment, which could be heard through a slightly ajar door and reverberating through the walls of the laboratory (Voice Feedback). A third condition had the victim in the same room as the Teacher about 1½ feet away, so his screams of pain could be heard, as well as be seen writhing in his chair (Proximity). The fourth condition had the victim receive the shock through an electrified plate; meaning beyond 150 volts the Teacher had to force down the Learner’s resisting hand to administer further punishment (Touch-Proximity).
Unsurprisingly, obedience dropped the more contact the Teacher had with the Learner: 34% defiance in the Remote condition, 37.5% with Voice Feedback, 60% in Proximity, and 70% in Touch-Proximity. The original graph is displayed below. Despite this drop in obedience, it should still be noted a worrying 30% of subjects would still physically force a protesting innocent to receive a potentially fatal electric shock.
In light of these findings,
it’s not so hard to imagine mass murder committed by soldiers under orders. We could also consider the increased ease at
which we can inflict pain and suffering on our fellow man during war time:
pressing a button to lauch a chemical strike on a village several hundred miles
away is significantly easier than running one man through with a sword in
hand-to-hand combat.
Stuart Miller
References
Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioural study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,
67(4), 371-378.
Milgram, S. (1965). Some conditions of obedience and
disobedience to authority. Human
Relations, 18(1), 57-76.
Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience
to Authority. New York: Harper & Row.
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.