CHANDLER, J., Griffin, T. M., & Sorensen, N.
(2008).
Katrina
Petrillo raised over $1000 for Hurraicane Katrina victims by selling lemonade.
When asked why, she said ‘I realised my name is going to go down in history as
one of the biggest storms ever’ (Salkin, 2005).
This paper
investigates the compliance technique of similarity in predicting that donors
who share an initial with a disaster will be more likely to donate to disaster
relief efforts for that particular disaster than would individuals who do not
share an initial with a disaster.
This
prediction, based on similarities affecting judgement and choice, revolves
around the theory of ‘unit-relation’ (Heider, 1958), whereby individuals
facilitate the assimilation of positive feelings of the self into the
representation of the target (Pelham et al, 2002), in an effort to avoid
associations with negative stimuli (Jones et al, 2004).
This
unit-relation undeniably involves the effect if similarity. One study found
that people with names beginning with the letters ‘D’ or ‘L’ are overrepresented
in the profession of dentists and lawyers respectively (Pelham et al, 2002) and
another that people named ‘George’ are more likely to live in Georgia than
expected by chance (Nuttin, 1987).
In order to
test the predictions, the researchers looked to sixty-six thousand donations
from 1930 to early 2006 made to the Red Cross, looking in particular at
Hurricane Katrina, which occurred in August 2005. They found that donations to
the Red Cross increased dramatically following Hurricane Katrina. There were
240 donations recorded in August 2005 (prior to Katrina), compared to an
average of 289 over the previous five Augusts (1999-2004).
Incorporating
the specificity of similarity in sharing an initial with hurricane Katrina
increasing the likelihood that one would donate to relief efforts, analyses
showed a pronounced name letter effect. Researchers examined the effect of time
(before vs. after Katrina) on disaster donations and where people allocated
their money after Katrina. 4.2% of the donors in the six months
prior to Katrina had a name with the initial K; this proportion increased to
over 5% (a 31% increase over baseline) after Katrina.
Considering four other hurricanes
(Charlie, August 2004; Francis, September 2004; Ivan, September 2004; and
Wilma, October 2005), same-initial donations increasing after each hurricane
proved significant. However, whilst supporting the prediction, the effect size
were statistically non-significant, most likely due to small sample sizes. A breakdown of the percentage increases for
each hurricane are shown below.
Conclusively, sharing an initial with a natural disaster increases the likelihood that people will donate to relief efforts for that disaster, but does not increase the average size of the donation. This finding replicates and extends earlier word done by Nelson and Simmons (2007) that suggests that people can be attracted to negative targets with which they share an initial. However, unlike previous studies, the findings from this experiment cannot easily be accounted for by the assimilation of positive feelings about the self into the target of evaluation. Current theories on this implicit egotism predict that people should either avoid association with a negative stimulus (Snyder et al, 1986) or downplay the negativity of same initial stimuli (Finch & Cialdini, 1989).
Lakhita Uppal
References
Chandler, J.,
Griffin, T. M., & Sorensen, N. (2008). In the ‘I’ of the storm: Shared
initials increase disaster donations.
Judgement and Decision Making, 3, 404-410.
Finch, J. F.,
& Cialdini, R. B. (1989). Another indirect tactic of (self-) image
management: Boosting. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 15, 222-232.
Heider, F.
(1958). The psychology of interpersonal
relations. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
Nelson, L.
D., & Simmons, J. P. (2007). Moniker Maladies: When names sabotage success.
Psychological Science, 18, 1106-1112.
Nuttin, J. M.
(1987). Affective consequences of mere ownership: The name letter effect in
twelve European languages. European
Journal of Social Psychology, 17, 381-402.
Pelham, B.
W., Mirenberg, M. C., & Jones, J. T. (2002). Why Susie sells seashells by
the seashore: Implicit egotism and major life decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 469-487.
Salkin, A.
(2005) What’s in a Name, Katrinas? New
York Times, p. 9-1.
Snyder, C.
R., Lassegard, M., & Ford, C. E. (1986). Distancing after group success and
failure. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 51, 382-388.
Good, id also say that association is important in this context too.
ReplyDelete