‘While stocks last’, ‘Sale ends soon’, we’ve all observed
shops warning us that products are of limited availability, attempting to lure
us in to purchase their endless supposed deals. TV shopping channels spend
little time telling us about the item, instead bombarding us with endless
warnings that the product won’t be on sale for much longer.
Brock (1968) referred to it as the commodity theory,
whilst Cialdini (2001) included as one of his six weapons of persuasion. Whilst
giving it different titles, they both referred to the same idea; we as
consumers want what we believe to be less available. The theory states that if
a commodity is scarce, or becoming scarcer, it is seen as more valuable, more
desirable.
This is understandable: as humans, we seek reasons for
why things happen. So if we notice that a certain commodity is less available
than another, we often determine that there must be a reason for this, and
often the reason we arrive it is that is must be popular, and therefore better
than the alternatives with unlimited quantity.
Numerous researchers have demonstrated this scarcity
effect. However, is there any more depth to this? Does the reason for its
scarceness determine whether it’s still seen as more desirable?
Verhallen and Robben (1994) investigated this, looking at
whether the reasons for the low availability would determine the preference for
a recipe book, and whether the social situation the participant found
themselves it had any effect on this.
Verhallen and Robben (1994) asked participants to rate
and choose one of 3 recipe books. Within this, there was two main conditions,
one where the participants were lead to believe that their preferences of
recipe book had an effect on other peoples choices (Social constraint
condition), and another whether there was no social constraint at all and they
were told they were the last ones to pick.
Additionally there were further conditions which were
about the availability of the different recipe books. Either the books could in
unlimited supply, or supply was limited. Within the limited condition there
were different reasons for scarcity; it could be due to accidental
circumstances, popularity, limited supply, or popularity and limited supply
together. The final two (either separately, or combined) are termed market
causes.
After testing 120 people, results of this experiment
showed that the reasons for an items scarcity do have an effect on choice. For
instance, if we look at table 1 below,
items which were of limited availability due to market reasons (popularity,
limited supply, or both of these combined) were significantly more likely to be
ranked as the first choice of book than the items which were either of limited
availability due to accidental circumstances, or that had unlimited availability.
This finding, whilst interesting, was to be expected as it is fundamentally the
scarcity principle as told by Brock, (1968).
An interesting finding within this experiment comes
from comparing the social constraint condition with the no social constraint
one. Though it was found that items with availability limited due to market
causes are of greater preference in the no social constraint group, this effect
was seen to disappear in the social constraint group. That is no significant
relationship is observed. This demonstrates that the presence of others when
making a choice will affect that choice, altering what we would perhaps have
otherwise preferred based on the availability information present.
Scarcity sells. If we want to increase consumer desire
for our product it is clear from the research that the idea that if they do
not purchase it quickly it will no longer be available causes an increase in
consumer desire for the item. However, this experiment would appear to suggest
that social constraints can affect our decision even if the item is scarce.
References:
Brock, C. (1968). Implications of commodity theory for value
change. In A. G. Greenwald, T. C. Brock, & T. M. Ostrom (Eds.), Psychological
foundations of altitudes. New York: Academic Press.
Cialdini, R. B. (2001). Influence: Science and
Practice. Boston:
Allyn & Bacon.
Verhallen, T. M. M. & Robben, H. S. J. (1994). Scarcity and preference: An experiment on unavailability and product evaluation. Journal of Economic Psychology, 15, 315-331.
Aaron Chaloner
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.