When we
think of authority, it is common for our minds to become preoccupied with
negative thoughts. As a psychology student, this negative portrayal of authority
is be due the classic studies conducted by Milgram’s electric shock experiment and
Zimbardo’s prison experiment which portray “the Lucifer effect” of how good
people can turn bad. Research however, has shown that authority can often have
positive outcomes in terms of parental – child and peer relationships. These
positive outcomes may be mediated by the amount of reciprocity we experience.
Whilst the
majority of people were celebrating the turn of the 21st century,
four researchers, Laursen, Noack,
Wilder, & Williams’, were instead investigating
how our cultural contexts have shaped our interpersonal relationships with our
peers and parents. Laursen et al (2000) broke their overall aim down into four
goals. For the aim of this blog, I will only be focusing on whether reciprocity,
authority and closeness correspond across relationships with mothers, fathers
and peers and whether these patterns differ across their two chosen samples
(American and German adolescents).
Adolescents
from the two countries were split into high closeness or low closeness groups according
by their score on the relationship closeness inventory as can be seen in figure
1 (Berscheid et al, 1989). This inventory assessed the quality of their paternal,
maternal and peer relationships. Overall, it was the German adolescents that
reported higher levels closeness in regards to all 3 relationship types. It was
however found that German adolescents were closer to their mothers and peers
than their fathers and US adolescents were closest to friends, mothers and then
fathers. This can be explained by the negative, authoritative, discipline
provider role fathers tend occupy. However, this explanation becomes
complicated as mothers who were rated as closest by adolescents, were also the
most authoritative. Surely we should dislike our mother for being too
authoritative if this is the reason why we have a sense of dislike towards our father’s
right? Wrong. We are emotionally, physically and genetically tied into a lifelong
relationship with our parents. This means that we have to accept a degree of
authority from them. It therefore seems as though small doses of authority are
not negative. Darling et al (2007) however, found that adolescent’s willingness
to listen to authority varied depending on the context. The way the child had
been socialised and disciplined also determines how likely they are to obey authority
(Darling et al, 2007).
Figure 1. The differences in high or low closeness across the German and US sample in regards to relationships with mothers, fathers and peers.
Adolescent
peer relationships were instead found to be characterised by reciprocity.
Unlike the obligatory relationship we have with our parents, we can shut down
unrewarding friendships with our peers. Reciprocity is therefore necessary to
maintain cohesion and balance amongst peers. Other research has found that
reciprocal peer friendships are rewarding because they allow the adolescent to
have more control and dominance (Hunter, 1984). As a matter of fact, Laursen et al (2000)
found that higher the level of reciprocity corresponded to higher levels of
closeness. What is interesting is that Laursen et al (2000) found that if an
adolescent had a reciprocal and close relationship with their peers or mother,
they did not place much emphasis or effort in forming relationships with
others. It is almost as though having these few close bonds led these
adolescents to feel socially satisfied. Overall, it was the German adolescents
who reported higher positive associations in terms of closeness, reciprocity
and authority with friends and their parents.
The overall
findings therefore suggest that despite the society an adolescent is reared in,
there is some interconnectedness between authority, closeness and reciprocity
in terms of how we perceive others and form relationships.
Darling, N.,
Cumsille, P., Martinez, M. L. (2007). Adolescents’ as active agents in the socialisation
process: Legitimacy of parental authority and obligation to obey as predictors
of obedience. Journal of Adolescence, 30,
297 – 311.
Hunter, F.
T. (1984). Socialising procedures in parent-child and friendship relations
during adolescence. Developmental
Psychology, 20, 1092 – 1099.
Laursen, B.,
Noack, P., Wilder, D., & Williams, V. (2000). Adolescent perceptions of
reciprocity, authority, and closeness in relationships with mothers, fathers,
and friends. International Journal of
Behavioural Development, 24, 464 – 471.
Super interesting Nim, well done.
ReplyDelete