Why do
polar bears need saving?
Polar Bears
are under threat! Over the last 30 years, Sea Ice had decreased by 40%, majorly
disrupting the bears habitat (GreenPeace, (2018). The reduced living space
means that polar bears smaller breeding grounds, and diminished hunting
territories. They are officially classified as an endangered species. Currently,
there are only 22,000 polar bears left, but a further decline of 30% is
expected by 2050 (WWF, 2018). The
decline in polar bears means that the Arctic biodiversity and ecosystem are
also under threat; polar bears eat seals and seals eat fish. So, the immediate
effect of a decline in the bear population is more seals and subsequently less
fish. As if a salmon fillet isn’t expensive enough already! Steps must be taken
to protect this beautiful creature, its home and its ecosystem.
Environmental
protection organisations, like GreenPeace and WWF, are attempting to put
pressure on the Government to reduce climate change. However, their campaigns
are not getting enough support from the public. Unfortunately, it’s rather
difficult to get people to act against climate change, even if they agree with
the sentiment (ie saving polar bears). George Marshal (2015) argues that our
brains are wired to ignore threats that are uncertain and in the future, and so
people do not engage with the issue of climate change. This makes it rather
difficult to persuade people to change their behaviour in favour if climate
protection. Because our brains prevent us from getting invested in the issue of
climate change, we use excuses like ‘I don’t have the time’ to avoid action.
The result is that oil firms and fracking companies are free to destroy the
Arctic without much resistance.
What we
did and how we did it
The aim of
our project is to get more people involved in the fight against climate change,
simply by getting them to sign one petition in favour of Arctic Protection. This
is a low-risk behaviour which is quick to do and costs no money, making it our
best shot for maximum recruitment. We created a poster (shown below) that
provides some information about threats to the arctic and what can be done
about it. We hung this poster up around campus, primarily in the Student Union’s
food outlets, which several hundred people visit every week. This way, we could
target people who are waiting for their food and thus had little choice but to
engage with the poster. However, most people who interacted with our poster
were focused on their lunch, so reaching them via the central route of
persuasion (where a rational argument would be effective) was unlikely (Petty & Cacioppo, 1979). We decided something more than rational argument was
needed to get our distracted audience to engage with the issue.
Methods
of persuasion
First, to
get people to actually look at our poster, we included persuasion techniques
from MINDSPACE (Dolan et al, 2012) to draw people in. Chiefly, the Affect Heuristic (Slovic et al.,
2002) This is the idea that subjective notions of “goodness” & “badness” of
a stimulus, are often decided by the immediate feeling the stimulus gives us.
We used cute polar bears to persuade people of the ‘goodness’ of our scheme and
by using a particularly cute but also sad picture of polar bears we hoped to
induce a motivation to help. There are several findings that support the notion
that just the polar bears being cute would promote helping behaviours (or a
desire to help at least). Sherman et al. (2009) found that viewing very cute
images of young animals enhanced behavioural carefulness significantly more
than less cute images of grown up animals (as a result of the evolutionary
adaptation for caring in humans). Gunnthorsdottir (2001) conducted research
into specifically how animal cuteness affects the likelihood of their
conservation. She found that cuter images of animals (when compared to less
attractive images of the same animal) induced greater helping behaviours and
increased desire to conserve the species. She also found that if an animal is
simply framed as endangered, the ratings of the animals’ attractiveness
increase from their initial scores. Which is exactly what we did.
To get
people to choose to sign the petition, we used another persuasion technique
called extremeness aversion, or the compromise effect (Simonson & Tversky, 1992).
This works by presenting someone with 3 options, with the option you want them
to pick in the middle. Regardless of content of these options, the person more
likely to choose the middle option. This happens because people think of
themselves as ‘average’ and so go for the ‘average’ option. Context-dependent
choices such as this, have been shown to influence what size drink people
purchase (Sharpe et al. 2008). Instead
of drinks sizes, we wanted to present people with the issue of Arctic
destruction and give them 3 choices. 1) Do nothing, 2) Sign a petition, 3)
Donate Money. Because people want to avoid extreme options, they should go for
option 2. We used QR codes that direct the page to 1) ‘I can’t believe you’ve
done this’ gif ("Cantbelieveyoudonethis
Punch GIF - Cantbelieveyoudonethis Punch - Discover & Share GIFs",
2018) 2) the GreenPeace
Petition, or 3) WWF page to adopt a polar bear. We hoped that by combining the cute
picture of polar bears with statistics of their current level of endangerment
would increase eagerness to conserve them and their habitat. If this is true it
would then feasibly mean that more people choose either the petition or
donation (Slovic
et al., 2002). This also forced people into a situation where they had to make
a choice between doing something or doing nothing. Highlighting, that every day
that they do not act to save the Arctic they are making an active coice, not
abstaining from making a choice.
As a final
touch, we included some colour and value elements to cue system 1 biases
(Kahneman, 2015). We did this by including a sad face
above the “I do not care about polar bears” option, a happy face above the
“sign the petition option”, and an extremely happy face above the “adopt a
polar bear” option. Additionally, the colour of each bar has an impact on the
reader as well. The “I don’t care about polar bears” option is red, while the
“sign the petition” option is in yellow, and the “adopt the polar bear” option
is in green. These faces and colours represent universal symbols that are
recognized by any reader as bad/stop, happy/ either speed up or slow down, and
extremely happy/ go. This creates a perception about which option the reader is
“supposed” to pick, if they are to go along with societal norms. Furthermore,
on top of these pictures we’ve written “223089 people are already helping, why
aren’t you? What will you do?” This also helps add to the perception that
everyone else already cares about polar bears, and that in order to fit into
society you should care about polar bears too. According to Clapp &
McDonnell (2000), “the perception of the societal norm is a stronger predictor
of behaviour than the actual societal norm”. So as long as you make people
think that other people care about polar bears, even if they don’t, they will
be more likely to sign the petition or adopt a polar bear
How we
tracked the success of our project
Using QR
codes let us track the effectiveness of our project, by counting how many times
each QR was activated. The graph below shows the number or responses for each
QR code 3 weeks after the posters went up. We had a total of 1144 response, and
as we had hoped, the majority (51%) of activations were for the petition.
Whilst we can’t know if those people signed the petition, we know that they
engaged with the poster and therefore, the issue, which is an achievement in
itself. People who went for the ‘Do Nothing’ option, at least got a good laugh
out of the gif they were directed to.
http://exaj.co.uk/qrcode/results.php (This website shows the up-to-date
number of responses)
Our hope
for the future
The main
thing we wanted to achieve with this project was to get more people to engage
with the issues and consequences of climate change. By getting them to sign a
petition, hopefully we will have planted a seed for further and greater action.
The foot-in-the-door technique (Freedman & Fraser, 1966) shows that once
people agree to a small request, like signing a petition, they are more likely
to comply with greater requests, like donating money. Even if people do not
donate (perhaps because of limited means) once people have made the decision to
commit to an issue, they are likely to be consistent (Petrova et al., 2007),
and so will continue to support environmentalist campaigns, even if its just
signing petitions. These may seem like rather insignificant acts, but every
person that stands up for the issues encourages more people to stand up, until
there is enough of us to support greater action against climate change.
Authors: Avery Watters, Ella Hepburn and Greta Mohr
References
Cantbelieveyoudonethis Punch GIF - Cantbelieveyoudonethis
Punch - Discover & Share GIFs. (2018). Tenor. Retrieved 18 March 2018, from
https://tenor.com/view/cantbelieveyoudonethis-punch-gif-9605268
Clapp, J.,
& McDonnell, A. (2000). The relationship of perceptions of alcohol
promotion and peer drinking norms to
alcohol problems reported by college students. Journal of College Student Development, 44, 19-26
Dolan, P.,
Hallsworth, M., Halpern, D., King, D., Metcalfe, R., & Vlaev, I. (2012).
Influencing behaviour: The mindspace
way. Journal Of Economic Psychology, 33(1),
264-277.
Freedman, J.
L., & Fraser, S. C. (1966). Compliance without pressure: the
foot-in-the-door technique. Journal
of personality and social psychology, 4(2), 195.
Gunnthorsdottir, A. (2001). Physical attractiveness
of an animal species as a decision factor for its preservation. Anthrozoƶs,
14(4), 204-215.
Help protect the Antarctic Ocean. (2018).
Secure.greenpeace.org.uk. Retrieved 10 March 2018, from https://secure.greenpeace.org.uk/page/s/antarctic?source=wb&subsource=20171031ocwb01
Kahneman,
D. (2015). Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
Marshall, G.
(2015). Don't Even Think about It. New York: Bloomsbury Publishing USA.
Petrova, P.
K., Cialdini, R. B., & Sills, S. J. (2007). Consistency-based compliance
across cultures. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(1),
104-111.
Petty, R., & Cacioppo, J. (1979). Issue involvement can
increase or decrease persuasion by enhancing message-relevant
cognitive responses. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 37(10),
1915-1926.
Sharpe, K.,
Staelin, R., & Huber, J. 2008. Using Extremeness Aversion to Fight Obesity:
Policy Implications of Context
Dependent Demand. Journal Of Consumer
Research, 35(3), 406-422.
Sherman, G. D., Haidt, J., & Coan, J. A.
(2009). Viewing cute images increases behavioural carefulness. Emotion, 9(2), 282.
Simonson, I., & Tversky, A.
(1992). Choice in
context: Trade-off contrast and extremeness aversion. JMR, Journal of Marketing Research, 29(3), 281
Slovic, P.,
Finucane, M., Peters, E., & McGregor, D. G. (2002). The affect heuristic.
In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin,
& D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgement (397– 420). New York: Cambridge University Press.
WWF. (2018).
Polar Bears. Retrieved 10 March 2018, from https://www.wwf.org.uk/wildlife/polar- bears
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.