“Female-focused”
products tend to be, unsurprisingly, pink. But why is it normal to assume any
product related to the female gender has to be packaged and promoted in pink,
orange, or other such colours? This occurrence manifests across a variety of
products, therefore implying an inherent sexism embedded in contemporary
culture expressed commercially. Despite backlash companies get (I even found a
Daily Mail article on it, which says a lot), the ‘pinkification’ of products
continues to prevail.
Why do companies
ignore protests?
Quick and easy
way to attract women to products where percentage of consumers who are male
exceed female?
It is
interesting to note that some brands do not receive any criticism for their
‘pinkification’. One reason, perhaps, for allowing some brands, is because they
have been able to persuade consumers there is a legitimate reason for the
differing characterization of products. For example, simply stating the
difference between men and women can enable the justification of having two
separate products. For example, men tend to shave facial hair and less bodily
hair, whereas the opposite applies to women. This difference justifies a difference
in the products marketed.
Cognitive
heuristic- stereotyping
The stereotypes
emanated through ‘pinkification’ embody a heuristic used reflecting a “perception
of social reality” which enables people to efficiently process social interactions. This reduces cognitive load in the abundance
of information we receive from the world (Biernat et al, 2003).
Lukšík’s (2003) defines gender stereotypes as:
“fixed, simplified, rigid and
biased beliefs about a »appropriate and
adequate« behaviour of men and women and their
»appropriate
and adequate« traits and other psychical and
social qualities”.
Pinkified products befits this definition- society seems to have evolved
to support the notion that pink is appropriate for women. This norm
historically only arose in the 20th century. Prior to this, there
was no universal “gender-colour symbolism” (Paoletti, 2012). Babies don’t tend
to show a preference to pink- girl or boy, indicating a lack of biological
mechanisms driving the colour bias we see marketed. However, at 2, girls start
to show a pink preference and boys a pink avoidance, suggestive of a cultural
influence (LoBoue & DeLoache, 2011) and a tendency to stick to ingroup
behaviours once after application to the group, in this case gender.
Social Identity Theory
This suggests the success of pink products later in adult life has been
successful due to the societal promotion of pink as a female colour, and in
order to have a stable sense of self within a group, females buy the products.
In other words, in order to stay in their ingroup, which has been labelled to
be associated to pink, they buy pink products, as staying ingroup means
validation of its beliefs and behaviours. The ingroup bias also reaffirms
self-concept, which” derives
from his knowledge of his membership in a social group” (Tajfel, 1981). The
maintenance of self-concept is intrinsic to the social identity theory (Tajfel &
Turner, 1979), and posits the maximization of difference between groups when
there is a preference for one.
Availability heuristic
This benefits companies, who can use a drive to maintain ingroup
behaviours by driving up prices on gendered products despite the availability
of an indentical/similar neutral or male product. The Times found women pay 37%
more for equivalent products (Ellson, 2016). The strengthening of gender
stereotypes is not surprising considering the ubiquitity of reinforcement. For
example, Gallagher (2010) found up to 46% of the news portrayed gender
stereotypes. This can be said to promote the tendency to utilize the
availability heuristic (Tversky & Kahneman, 1973). This means the (pink)
products women often see comes to mind more likely than less frequently seen
products when deciding what to purchase. This is because it is easier to
retrieve. A classic study depicting the availability heuristic in action is
Tversky and Kahneman’s (1973) study, in which famous celebrities were recalled
more often than less famous celebrities. The tendency towards change which will
lower the number of encounters to stereotypical products, measured in
advertisements and broadcasting has been too low to make a considerable
difference in the way products are marketed (Bretl & Cantor, 1988; Lindner,
2004).
The arguments presented can be reflected in a variety of products and
services across the global market, from the same arguments explaining male blue
and black products, to the gender normal behaviour for females of cosmetics,
cosmetic surgery, and so on. And until there is mass boycotting of neutral
products that have become genderised by pink labels, they will be continued to
be produced.
There are some arguments postulating a “stupid surcharge” on pink
products, meaning free market consumers can choose what to buy, and women have
the choice to buy cheaper alternatives. This, of course, is true, but why
should we have to avoid the abundance of pinkified products if the same
products for men are cheaper anyway? No demand means no supply, so boycotting
these products may be the only means to ensure no unnecessary price difference
between products in the future. Also, on
a wider scale, pinkification leads to pointless differentiation between men and
women- should societies general promotion of binary gender really be implicated
into what shampoo you buy?
A free market argument can be adapted to say: buy whatever product you
like the most. Whether this should be a pink or blue razor is up to individual
preference, but the indisputable point remains that a price difference between
two items with the same function and ingredients/materials is reductionist in
the current culture of slowly gaining equality. Persuasion techniques should be
identified in this region so they can be overcome.
References:
Biernat, M., Kobrynowicz, D.,
& Weber, D. L. (2003). Stereotypes and Shifting Standards: Some Paradoxical
Effects of Cognitive Load. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33, 2060–2079.
Bretl, D. J., & Cantor, J. (1988).
The portrayal of men and women in U.S. television commercials: A recent content
analysis and trends over 15 years. Sex
Roles, 18, 595-609.
Ellson, A. (2016, January 19). Women
charged more on ‘sexist’ high street. Retrieved from https://www.thetimes.co.uk.
Lindner, K. (2004). Images of
women in general interest and fashion magazine advertisements from 1955 to
2002. Sex Roles, 51,409–20.
Lukšík, I. (2003). Gender
Stereotypes [in:] Upgrade for Sexual Education. Bratislava: VEDA Publishing
House.
LoBue, V., & DeLoache, J. S.
(2011). Pretty in pink: the early development of gender-stereotyped colour
preferences. British Journal of
Developmental Psychology, 29, 476-481.
Paoletti, J. (2012). Pink and Blue: Telling the Boys from the
Girls in America. Bloomington, Indiana: Indiana University Press.
Tajfel, H. (1981). Human
Groups and Social Categories. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge University Press.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C.
(1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. The social psychology
of intergroup relations?, 33, 47.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D.
(1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive
Psychology, 5, 207–232.
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.