Whilst familiarity with a product may prove advantageous
for some brands, Rowntree’s decided that this was boring. So for the launch of their innovative new
product, ‘Randoms’, they adopted a rather more absurd approach.
The most striking aspect of this advert is its
humour. Words expected to appear in a
list of directions are replaced with surprising ones related not to the problem
presented, but to the product being snacked on by the actor throughout the
ad. Interestingly, these words and the
objects they represent are totally commonplace and familiar (e.g. saxophone,
rubber duck); the surprise lies in their being unexpectedly mixed together.
Arias-Bolzmann, Chakraborty and Mowen (2000) might call this a ‘surreal’
or ‘allegory’ form of absurdity, as it combines objects in surprising ways and
uses imagery to reflect the quality of quirkiness. This is reinforced by the product itself
(comprising ‘random’ combinations of shapes, colours and textures), as well as
by the advert’s setting. Indeed,
the ad utilises a contrast technique (Herr, 1986): the greys of the road,
pavement, car and the woman’s business suit are contrasted with the colour of
the sweets and the shocking language. One
can almost hear the advert shouting, This
product is a refreshing change from the norm! Does it work?
Arias-Bolzmann et al. suggest that absurd adverts, due to their
attention grabbing, may be processed more elaborately and extensively (p.
37).
Humour has been linked with increased favour towards an advert
and the brand (Eisend, 2009), but how does it link? This
question opens a can of worms: does humour rely mostly on affective or cogntive
processes, leading to this attitude change?
Some researchers have pointed to
an ‘affect transfer’ effect (e.g. De Houwer, Thomas & Bayens, 2001) in
which the positive mood elicited by humour becomes associated with the target
(the ‘Randoms’, here). Contrarily, Petty and Cacioppo’s (1986) Elaboration
Likelihood Model (ELM) suggests that an individual placed in a good mood (e.g.
by humour) will be less likely to disagree with the message presented in an
advert and more likely to rely on ‘peripheral’ cues (aspects of the advert not
directly related to the product) rather than ‘central’ cues (those that are
related, such as the product’s merit). It
is worth noting here that there exist individual differences in the motivation
to engage in central, systematic processing of a persuasive message, known as ‘need
for cognition’ (NFC), wherein low NFC individuals are easier to persuade because
they tend towards peripheral processing (Zhang, 1996). Gender differences have been found with
respect to NFC, such that males seem to appreciate humour in advertising more,
and engage more in peripheral processing for humorous adverts (Chan,
2011). What does this mean? It suggests that this particular advert will
appeal the most to – and persuade more effectively – those who attend mostly to its
presentation of using humour.
It is arguable that Rowntree’s even go the extra mile
considering this phenomenon. The
principal character is similar in a number of ways to the product’s supposed target
audience: an ordinary – until he eats the sweets and starts to talk nonsense – (male)
person who appreciates humour and quirkiness (this being, at the time, a new, innovative product in its industry). Associative casting
(Pratkanis, 2007) might also be at work here, such that a viewer may desire to associate
him/herself with the good peripheral qualities presented (being funny and standing
out from the norm), so will be more likely to head out and buy the product, and this is made more effective by the use of similarity.
Ultimately, simple as this advert may appear, its use of surprise,
contrast, humour and audience-targeting make it simply effective as a product-launcher. With ‘Randoms’, Rowntree’s are almost
literally ‘breaking the mould’ in confectionary production (pun intended), and
with a parting tagline of, “Let your Random side out”, maybe you can too…
References
Arias-Bolzmann, L., Chakraborty, G., & Mowen, J. C.
(2000). Effects of absurdity in advertising: The moderating role of product category
attitude and the mediating role of cognitive responses. Journal of Advertising, 29, 35-49.
Chan, F. Y. (2011). Selling through entertaining: The
effect of humour in television advertising in Hong Kong. Journal of Marketing and Communications, 17, 319-336.
De Houwer, J., Thomas, S., & Bayens, F. (2001).
Association learning of likes and dislikes: A review of 25 years of research on
human affective conditioning. Psychological
Bulletin, 127, 853-869.
Eisend, M. (2009). A meta-analysis of humor in
advertising. Journal of the Academy of
Marketing Science, 37, 191-203.
Herr, P. M. (1986). Consequences of priming: Judgment and
behaviour. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 51, 1106-1115.
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The
elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. Advances
in Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 123-205.
Pratkanis, A. R. (2007). The science of social influence: Advances and future progress. Hove:
Psychology Press.
Zhang, Y. (1996). The effect of humor in advertising: An
individual-difference perspective. Psychology
& Marketing,13, 531-545.
Izzy Fawdry
Well done Izzy, I enjoyed reading this.
ReplyDelete