It works at home, in high profile businesses and even in retail
(Ebster & Neumayr, 2008) and all comes down the innate principle of
reciprocity. What Cialdini (2007) refers to as the larger-then-smaller-request technique, more widely known as the Door in the Face phenomenon. A person will be more willing to seal the deal when a more expensive
pricing is initially stated prior to a reduced, more moderate request. Two
facets contribute to this phenomenon: a change in perception of the value of
money, and the human instinct of reciprocity.
Let’s look at “Motherfucker Jones’s” request (see video
above). He sets a price on being the mind behind the murder of the three bosses - $50,000 – once rejected, he immediately replaces it
with a moderate price - $600. Now, surely, such an instant second request was
planned? Well, by suggesting $50,000, he knows for sure that the latter request
of $600 will instinctively be valued as cheaper and a more reasonable price. And
so it sells. What makes this technique fascinating is the concept that if $600
were put forward as the initial request, inherently its value would
perceptually become more deer. This strategy distorts the customer’s perception
of the value of money and increases the willingness to spend it. By comparing
to the earlier price, the latter price – although still expensive – appears smaller.
A scenario of my own occurred 4 years ago in a club in the
south of France. The bartender proposed I pay 8 euros for a can of red bull. Of
course, I was stunned by this price, so I decided to what some might call ‘trick’ the bartender by using the Door in the Face technique from the
customer’s perspective. At first, I tried to barter – ‘6 euros?’. He was having
none of it. So I decided to test whether he would notice the complete illogical
nature of the next proposal I made – ‘2 for 8 euros?’. For a reason that was
unknown to me back then, he accepted. I had managed to get 2 cans of red bull
for 8 euros, 1 can for 4 euros – half the price initially proposed AND less
than the first, rejected, offer I had made (6 euros)! The question many people
would ask now is “How did he not realise this outrageous loss in money?!”. Well,
it all comes down to this implicit compliance technique working
via the theory of reciprocity. The bartender may have felt, after rejecting my
initial offer of 6 euros, that he owes something, perhaps a concession for
the next offer (2 for 8 euros). This step to compliance may not have been
conscious, but it occurs with or without intention.
Yet, could it be that the moment he heard me offer 8 euros the
bartender sealed the deal out of impulsivity? Having initially requested that
price, once I had conformed to that, he may have assumed it was a break through
without processing the complications of actually losing money for what was sold
(2 red bull cans rather than 1).
Let’s turn to
another explanation. It’s not alien to us that a reduced price elicits the
assumption that we are ‘gaining’ money when accepting a smaller request.
Take a dress on offer for example, reduced from $500 to $250. For most, the automatic response
is “I’m saving $250, so I have to buy this dress!”. Most certainly, you are NOT
saving $250. If anything, losing a pretty large amount of money. Surely we are
aware of the irrationality behind our reasoning when buying reduced items? So
why do we still do it? Does it make us feel better? The answer is yes and what
Festinger’s (1957) theory of cognitive dissonance might argue as our
instinctive way of sustaining attitudes and behaviours as consistent. Buying
the dress (the behaviour) and simultaneously holding the attitude that it is
too expensive results in a cognitive conflict. Something has to change, and so
we make excuses for our behaviour (clearly buying the dress is more important).
Consequently, we convince ourselves that the dress is cheap. In extreme terms,
we excuse our behaviour by persuading ourselves that we are ‘saving’ or ‘gaining’
money.
To conclude,
Tips for all hagglers:
1)
Starting prices must be higher than within the
reasonable range – that way, a smaller yet still expensive price will appear
cheaper and may sell.
2)
Emphasise the point that the second moderate
price constitutes a concession – people feel guilty and WILL reciprocate for
that.
3)
Sell your credibility – this goes without
saying.
References:
Cialdini, R. B.
(2007). Influence: The Psychology of Persuasion. New York: Collins.
Ebster, C., &
Neumayr, B. (2008). Applying the door-in-the-face compliance technique to
retailing. The International Review of Retail, Distribution and Consumer
Research, 18(1), 121-128.
Festinger, L.
(1957). A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford
University Press.
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.