Whether you were interested in the outcome of the 2015 General Elections
or not, you will probably have heard the sentence “We have a long-term
economic plan that’s working”, spoken by the Prime Minister and his party. Why
did this sentence stand out? Purely and simply because it was constantly repeated. The Prime Minister and his fellow Members of Parliament would try and slide this phrase into most of the answers they gave to questions raised by the public and the media.
After watching many television debates, we had heard this catch-phrase more times than we could count, and most Conservative voters were starting to find it
fairly painful to watch their leader use his favourite debate. It seemed as though their party’s leader was getting
complacent, perhaps because the leader of the opposition was struggling to
build a reputation following his recent attempt at eating a bacon sandwich.
Labour supporters probably couldn’t believe their luck—after
all, with a country crying out for more relatable politicians and complaining
of feeling disengaged with politics, why would any prospective leader try to
put the voter off by repeating the same sentence over and over again like a broken record?
The truth is, this endless repetition of “We have a long-term
economic plan” was very deliberate. Having been hired by the Conservative party
to run their election campaign, Lynton Crosby, an election strategist for
centre-right parties, and Jim Messina, Barack Obama’s former aide, instructed
all members of the party to repeat this sentence, in order to send out a clear
message about past economic successes and the ability to maintain these
successes in future (Watt, 2015). And here was what was key: regardless of how embarrassing
it might be, they would have to repeat this simple message as much as possible.
Of course, this would never be enough to convince someone from
the opposition to change sides. On the contrary, it most probably led them to believe that the Conservatives were on a path to destruction. The targets were swing voters, whose opinions
were likely to be changed more easily through the use of simple, consistent
messages. The result? A surprising Conservative majority, in spite of neck-and-neck opinion polls and mixed television debate ratings.
Why did this work?
Why did this work?
A well-known effect called the Mere Exposure Effect, first
discovered by Zajonc (1968) states that people develop attitudes that favour things
that they have been exposed to more, without having had any interaction with those things. This effect has been reproduced with exposure to people, visual stimuli and noises. Research has
also found this same effect when exposing participants to political statements. The difference with these is that people tend to feel very strongly about certain issues, so one would imagine it might be harder to change people's attitude to these issues by doing something as simple as exposing them to a particular argument more.
In an experiment by Miller (1976), students’ attitudes towards reducing foreign
aid were measured after varying degrees of exposure to posters condoning the
reduction of foreign aid in order to balance the government’s spending.
The results suggested that exposing participants to a political, persuasive message was enough to increase positive attitudes towards it. The effects do show that over-exposing participants to a message somewhat dampened the positive effect on their attitudes, but was still better than no exposure at all. This could possibly explain how the constant message repetition that the Conservatives stuck to might have been part of the reason for the vote-swing favouring them. Perhaps they should have found a middle-ground when it came to repeating, and therefore exposing their catch-phrase to the general public, for optimal effects, but it seems that there is reason to believe that attitudes towards their "long-term economic plan" were improved through the repetition of this message.
References:
Miller, R.L. (1976). Mere exposure, psychological reactance, and attitudinal change. Public Opinion Quarterly, 40, 229-243.
Watt, N. (2015). Tory strategists enforce rigid discipline as election approaches. The Guardian. Retrieved from:
http://www.theguardian.com/politics/2015/feb/03/tory-strategists-enforce-rigid-discipline-lynton-crosby
Zajonc, R.B, (1968). The attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Monograph Supplement, 9, 1-27.
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.