This advert attempts to persuade the audience to use soya
products (as an alternative to eating meat), by outlining the various benefits
of soya-based food products. The title of the poster features a play on words
as a means to grab the audience’s attention. Perceptual contrast to increase
compliance is employed in this advert. The poster features two sources: a
relatively unattractive man wearing everyday attire and an attractive woman
wearing a lab coat. The former argues for a position in which people should eat
meat (an opposite to the position that this poster advocates), whereas the
latter provides counter arguments to explain why soya-based products are a better
alternative. The rationale behind this contrast between sources is that the
presence of an unattractive source would increase audience’s perceptions of
attractive source’s attractiveness, thereby increasing the persuasive influence
often accompanied with the physical attractiveness bias.
Regarding the use of wordplay in the above advertisement,
research has shown that the use of puns (as well as their relevancy to the
advert) can influence the extent to which targets consider slogans funny. Kenrick and Gutierres (1980) presented subjects with 24 different slogans, which fell into three
categories: those that did not contain a pun, those that contained a pun with
one relevant meaning, and those containing a pun with two relevant meanings.
Subjects were asked to rate the extent to which they felt the puns were ‘well-chosen’
and ‘pleasing’. It was revealed that slogans containing a pun were better
appreciated than those that did not. In addition, slogans with with two
relevant interpretations were considered better than those with only one, and
were thus considered to be ‘better chosen’. Given this finding, it seems
possible that the appreciation of such slogans could extend to the adverts in
which they are featured, thereby increasing their persuasive influence on the
audience.
There is research to show that physical attractiveness can
enhance the persuasive influence of a communicator. In a field experiment,
Chaiken (1979) had several communicators approach university students on campus
and ask them to fill out an opinion survey (that the University should stop
serving meat in the common rooms), as well as supporting the survey’s position
with their own brief arguments. On this survey, targets indicated their
agreement along with their perceptions of the communicator’s friendliness,
knowledgeability, and attractiveness. Subsequent analysis of the data revealed
that attractive communicators yielded greater agreement form targets than did
unattractive communicators. In addition, it was revealed that targets perceived
attractive communicators as friendlier than unattractive ones.
As mentioned, the advert contains an attractive communicator
and an unattractive one, with the latter of the two conveying the advert’s persuasive
message. In a field study by Kenrick and Gutierres (1980), groups of male
students who had watched a TV show with three attractive female protagonists
were asked to judge the attractiveness of an average-looking female (shown in a
photo). It was found that, compared with control groups, those who had watched
the TV show beforehand gave the lowest ratings of attractiveness. The findings
here suggest an effect of perceptual contrast – that is, when presented with
two stimuli that contrast on a certain quality (i.e. physical attractiveness),
people have a tendency to view the disparity between those two stimuli in a
more extreme manner than is usual. When applied to the concept of physically
attractive communicators in advertising, it seems plausible that having an
unattractive communicator alongside an attractive one could increase audience
perceptions of the latter’s attractiveness, therefore leading to an enhanced persuasive
effect of the physical attractiveness bias. In addition, the contrast in attire
between each communicator in the above advert could possibly lead to a
heightened audience perception of the latter’s expertise (although this extends
beyond the domain of attractiveness explored by Kenrick and Gutierres).
The refutation of counterarguments in an advert can help to
take away the target’s excuse for not complying with a request. One such compliance
tactic that uses this rationale is the ‘defusing objections technique’, which
involves acknowledging counterarguments and then refuting them. Pardini and
Katzev (1986) found that using this technique to defuse common objections for
energy conservation in homeowners actually lead to increased support for it
(compared to controls).
In the domain of persuasion and influence, source
credibility refers to aspects of the communicator that audiences perceive and
use to judge their trustworthiness as messengers of a persuasive message. One
aspect that can affect such perceptions is the attire of the communicator.
Several studies have demonstrated that clothing associated with authority or
expertise (e.g. a police officer’s uniform or a scientist’s lab coat) can have
an impact on behavioural compliance. In one of Milgram’s studies on obedience,
the experimenter wore a lab coat (as symbol of their expertise and authority).
One manipulation of this study had the experimenter replaced by a person
wearing everyday clothes, which lead to a significant drop in participant obedience.
Overall, this study illustrates the way in which symbols of expertise can lead
to an increase in behavioural compliance. Furthermore, Cialdini (1993)
explained that symbols of expertise (such as the uniform of a doctor) can place the target of influence into the role of “unknowing public”. That is, the
communicator’s ornaments of expertise implicitly serve to highlight the target’s
own lack of knowledge on a certain matter, making it more likely that the
target is receptive to information and arguments form that perceived expert.
References (used in advert)
Anderson, J. W., Johnstone, B. M., & Cook-Newell, M. E. (1995). Meta-analysis of the effects of soy protein intake on serum lipids. New England Journal of Medicine, 333(5), 276-282.
Sadler, M. J. (2004). Meat alternatives—market developments and health benefits. Trends in Food Science & Technology, 15(5), 250-260.
References (used below advert)
Chaiken, S. (1979).
Communicator physical attractiveness and persuasion.Journal
of Personality and social Psychology, 37(8), 1387.
Cialdini, R. B. (1993).
Influence: The psychology of persuasion. New York: Morrow.
Kenrick, D. T., & Gutierres,
S. E. (1980). Contrast effects and judgments of physical attractiveness: When
beauty becomes a social problem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38(1), 131.
Milgram, S. (1963).
Behavioral study of obedience. The Journal of abnormal and social psychology, 67(4), 371.
Pardini, A. U., &
Katzev, R. D. (1986). Applying full-cycle social psychology to consumer
marketing: The defusing objections technique. Journal of economic psychology, 7(1), 87-94.
Van Mulken, M., Van
Enschot-van Dijk, R., & Hoeken, H. (2005). Puns, relevance and appreciation
in advertisements. Journal of pragmatics, 37(5), 707-721.
| |
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.