2016
has been a memorable year across the globe, from Brexit to Protest against
South Korean President, and from the Turkish coup d'état attempt to the Nice
attack. Somehow Trump became the President of the US by winning 278 electoral
votes, but how come Hilary Clinton did not win the election despite having so
many celebrity endorsements?
Figure 1. Michelle Obama, Barack Obama, ex-competitor Bernie Sanders, Katy Perry supporting Hilary Clinton.
Figure 2. Leonardo
DiCaprio, Beyonce, Lady Gaga, George
Clooney supporting Hilary Clinton.
Figure 3. Kendall Jenner, Miley Cyrus, Kim Kardashian, Kanye West, Oprah supporting Hilary Clinton.
As shown above, Hilary Clinton was able
to get support from both celebrities and famous politicians. Research indicates
that celebrity endorsements have positive and effective impact on preference
from the audience (Dean and Biswas, 2001; Silvera, & Austad, 2004), and
celebrities are viewed as highly trustworthy, believable, and persuasive in
terms of endorsing the targets (Freiden, 1984). Curiously, with all the celebrity
supporting Clinton, why didn’t she win? Well, the reason is partly due
to the 'Just-Plain-Folks' propaganda and peripheral route in the elaboration
likelihood model.
1. 'Just-Plain-Folks'
propaganda
Politicians often
act as plain folks rather than a posh and wealthy congressman, in order to
communicate the message of ‘I am just like you and I understand you’ across to
the audience that they want the votes from. When presidential candidates
act ordinarily, down-to-earth and participate in
normal activities,
it gives the voters a sense of trust and comfort, believing that the candidate
and the voters share common grounds and they therefore should agree with the
candidate.
People vote for political candidates that
they feel empathic towards (McCue & Gopoian, 2000), and people are more likely to show
empathic concerns and helping behaviour to someone who they believe are similar
to them (Batson, Duncan, Ackerman, Buckley
& Birch, 1981). In the experiment, when the participants are told that the
sufferer had similarities in attitudes, they chose to offer help and replace
the confederate that was given electric shocks, even in situations where
participants could easily leave without being irritated and stressed by
observing the sufferer’s painful appearance. If people felt that they are
similar to Clinton, they would have been more empathic towards her, and hence
potentially helped her by voting for Clinton in the election.
Figure 4. Results from Batson, Duncan, Ackerman, Buckley & Birch (1981).
However, seeing
celebrities posting pictures with Hilary Clinton on multiple social media
sites, voters tend to gain the impression that Hilary Clinton is another one of
those wealthy politicians instead of a more practical plain folk comparable to voters,
therefore the empathy/similarity votes that could’ve belonged to Clinton went
to Trump (or maybe not).
2. Elaboration
likelihood model plays a big part in the election
Campaigns fundamentally change voters’
decisions and propaganda techniques prime people’s view on candidates (Iyengar
& Simon, 2000; Druckman, 2004). To determine candidate
liking and vote choice, voters' perceptions of character and personal attributes have large impact
on the vote (Aylor, 1999). Also, voters evaluate presidential candidates on the basis of a set of general criteria,
which they use to judge the candidates’ personal attributes before voting (Miller et al., 1986). Stokes (1966) argued
that personality best explains for shifts in the vote from one presidential
election to the next, which is an example of voters utilizing peripheral route
of persuasion in the elaboration likelihood model (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986). When voters go down the peripheral route of persuasion, they use cues such as characteristics and attractiveness of the candidate to make the voting decision, and decisions are generally unrelated to the logics of the presidential speech or partisan quality.
Figure 5. Results from Aylor (1999).
Surprisingly, research
has shown that highly educated individuals are more prone to use the peripheral route
by focusing more on personal attributes of the candidate and using personality
categories rather than the candidate quality to make the decision, compared to
less educated individuals (Glass, 1985; Miller et al.,
1986). The reason might be because educated individuals view political
elections sceptically, as policy making depends not only on the president. Hence,
educated individuals would pay more attention to the candidates'
personal attributes as they give a true and clear picture of the potential
president.
Figure 7. Results from Glass (1985).
Therefore, Trump’s very ‘strong’ personality
attracts voters’ attention, and those who were using the peripheral route of persuasion would ignore
the candidate quality of Trump, despite some of his messages and opinions on
multiple issues were disrespectful and outrageous. In other words, if voters were attracted by Trump's personal attributes and his unique enthusiasm, they would have forgotten about Clinton and supported Trump instead, and it seems like they have.
Although Hilary Clinton did a good job
getting support from all of her celebrity friends, these factors might have
prevented her from becoming the president. Next time she runs for presidency,
it is best to remind her of taking a Behaviour Change course before any
campaign begins.
Sijia Zhou (Katie)
References
Aylor, B.
(1999). Source credibility and presidential candidates in 1996: The changing
nature of character and empathy
evaluations. Communication Research Reports, 16(3),
296-304.
Batson, C.
D., Duncan, B. D., Ackerman, P., Buckley, T., & Birch, K. (1981).
Is empathic emotion a source of
altruistic motivation?. Journal of personality and Social Psychology, 40(2),
290.
Dean, D. H.,
& Biswas, A. (2001). Third-party organization endorsement of products:
An advertising cue affecting
consumer prepurchase evaluation of goods and services. Journal of
Advertising, 30(4), 41-57.
Druckman, J. N. (2004). Priming the vote: Campaign effects in
a US Senate
election. Political Psychology, 25(4), 577-594.
Freiden, J.
B. (1984). Advertising spokesperson effects-An examination of endorser
type and gender on 2
audiences. Journal of advertising research, 24(5),
33-41.
Glass, D. P.
(1985). Evaluating presidential candidates: Who focuses on their personal
attributes?. Public
Opinion Quarterly, 49(4), 517-534.
Iyengar, S.,
& Simon, A. F. (2000). New perspectives and evidence on political
communication and campaign
effects. Annual review of psychology, 51(1), 149-169.
McCue, C. P., & Gopoian, J. D. (2000). Dispositional
empathy and the political gender
gap. Women & Politics, 21(2),
1-20.
Miller, A. H., Wattenberg, M. P., & Malanchuk, O. (1986).
Schematic assessments of
presidential candidates. American
Political Science Review, 80(02), 521-540.
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration
likelihood model of persuasion.
In Communication
and persuasion (pp. 1-24). Springer New York.
Silvera, D. H., & Austad, B. (2004). Factors predicting
the effectiveness of celebrity
endorsement
advertisements. European Journal of marketing, 38(11/12),
1509-1526.
Stokes, D. E. (1966). Some dynamic elements of contests for
the presidency. American
Political
Science Review, 60(01), 19-28.
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.