Our project focused on getting more young people,
predominately in the Millennial generation (Generation Y) to adopt a saving
mind-set through a £1 a day challenge for four weeks. This was conducted to
make Millennials feel more positive about saving money, and highlight that
saving need not mean compromising on all aspects of life.
First, who exactly are
Millennials (Generation Y)?
Born between 1980 and the late 1990s, they are the generation that grew up during the 2007 recession, but also grew up with technology. The cut-off point is somewhat debated however, with the Oxford English Dictionary (2018) providing a fairly broad definition of Millennials as “Denoting people reaching adulthood in the early 21st century”.
It is the biggest cohort in US history with 92 million Millennials compared to 61 million Gen X and 77 million Baby Boomers (US Census Bureau, 2015), and in 2016 Millennials accounted for about 14% of the UK’s population (Brown et al., 2017).
In general, Millennials tend to have a bad reputation and are often labelled as ‘entitled’ and the ‘me, me, me generation’. However, Millennials are more likely to spend their money on sustainability and ethically sourced goods (Nielson, 2015) than previous generations.
Born between 1980 and the late 1990s, they are the generation that grew up during the 2007 recession, but also grew up with technology. The cut-off point is somewhat debated however, with the Oxford English Dictionary (2018) providing a fairly broad definition of Millennials as “Denoting people reaching adulthood in the early 21st century”.
It is the biggest cohort in US history with 92 million Millennials compared to 61 million Gen X and 77 million Baby Boomers (US Census Bureau, 2015), and in 2016 Millennials accounted for about 14% of the UK’s population (Brown et al., 2017).
In general, Millennials tend to have a bad reputation and are often labelled as ‘entitled’ and the ‘me, me, me generation’. However, Millennials are more likely to spend their money on sustainability and ethically sourced goods (Nielson, 2015) than previous generations.
So, are Millennials
really bad savers then?
Yes, and no is the answer. Millennials are more conscious than previous generations about personal health, with more money spent on fitness and healthier foods. For example, sales in pre-packaged baby foods have declined by 14% from 2012 to 2015, with more parents opting for homemade alternatives (Mintel, 2016). Additionally, a survey on over 2000 students revealed that on average they spent £87 more per month on fitness, such as gym memberships and exercise classes, than students that graduated 1997-2017 (SPCE, 2017).
Research by Gallup (Fleming, 2017) revealed that in comparison to all other generations, Millennials go online to compare prices to find the best deals 16% more, are 14% more likely to purchase used goods, and just as likely to purchase generic/store-brand goods. However, Millennials reported 13% more impulse purchases, went shopping for fun 13% more and were 5% less likely to use coupons in comparison to other generations.
Thus, millennial spending habits are not clear cut, with good saving habits formed, but also evidence of maladaptive spending habits also.
Yes, and no is the answer. Millennials are more conscious than previous generations about personal health, with more money spent on fitness and healthier foods. For example, sales in pre-packaged baby foods have declined by 14% from 2012 to 2015, with more parents opting for homemade alternatives (Mintel, 2016). Additionally, a survey on over 2000 students revealed that on average they spent £87 more per month on fitness, such as gym memberships and exercise classes, than students that graduated 1997-2017 (SPCE, 2017).
Research by Gallup (Fleming, 2017) revealed that in comparison to all other generations, Millennials go online to compare prices to find the best deals 16% more, are 14% more likely to purchase used goods, and just as likely to purchase generic/store-brand goods. However, Millennials reported 13% more impulse purchases, went shopping for fun 13% more and were 5% less likely to use coupons in comparison to other generations.
Thus, millennial spending habits are not clear cut, with good saving habits formed, but also evidence of maladaptive spending habits also.
So why target Millennials then?
As a generation, Millennials are the first generation predicted to be worse off than their parent’s generation (Corlett, 2017). This is in part due to the growing gap between income and house prices i.e. the housing crisis, increasing student loan debt, and increasing costs of living in general.
In 1996, 55% of households were led by someone 25-29 years old, and 68% of 30-34 year olds were owner occupiers. In comparison, in 2017 59% of Millennial-led households were renting (Brown et al., 2017).
The income to housing price gap is rising also (as depicted by Graph 1). On average, in 2016 those who work could be expected to spend around 7.6 times their yearly income on purchasing a home within England and Wales, this is up from an average of 3.6 times ones yearly income in 1997 (Office for National Statistics, 2017).
As a generation, Millennials are the first generation predicted to be worse off than their parent’s generation (Corlett, 2017). This is in part due to the growing gap between income and house prices i.e. the housing crisis, increasing student loan debt, and increasing costs of living in general.
In 1996, 55% of households were led by someone 25-29 years old, and 68% of 30-34 year olds were owner occupiers. In comparison, in 2017 59% of Millennial-led households were renting (Brown et al., 2017).
The income to housing price gap is rising also (as depicted by Graph 1). On average, in 2016 those who work could be expected to spend around 7.6 times their yearly income on purchasing a home within England and Wales, this is up from an average of 3.6 times ones yearly income in 1997 (Office for National Statistics, 2017).
Aims of our project
Despite financial difficulty ahead, we wanted to
emphasise to Millennials that it may not be fair, but saving regularly from an
earlier age, even in small amounts is better than not saving at all. Research has shown that people create habits early towards money. For example if businesses pay tax on time in their first year, they will be highly likely to continue, as they have set up the necessary habits such as good record keeping (Halpern, 2016). Interventions for getting businesses to pay tax on time become less successful with each year of the business. Therefore, students may not think about saving money
now as they have their student loan. This may create a dangerous ‘non-saver’ mind-set
in the future. Thus, we are aiming to create a pro-saver mind set from an
earlier age which hopefully will be adopted later in their lives also. Another
focus is to make Millennials aware that saving need not impinge on life
satisfaction, an important concept for this generation. Research has shown that
more than any other generation Millennials are more likely to spend on
experiences, rather than material possessions, and put heavy weight on life
satisfaction also (SPCE, 2017).
Attribution
We chose the name ‘Savvy Saver Challenge’ for our
Facebook Page, and campaign in general. We actively chose to include the concept
of ‘saver’ rather than ‘savings’. Research by Bryan, Monin and Adams (2012)
found that subtle linguistic differences caused participants to change their
behaviour. Participants were less likely to cheat if presented with language
that highlighted the implications of cheating for the participant’s identity i.e.
‘Please don’t be a cheater’ compared to when language focused on the action
instead i.e. ‘Please don’t cheat’. Other research by Miller, Brickman, Bolen
(1975) found that attributing behaviour to oneself influences people to maintain
attitude-congruent behaviour (recycling). In this particular study, school classes
were put into different recycling conditions: Control, Attribution and Persuasion.
In the persuasion group the class were predominately told about throwing away
rubbish and had a ‘Don’t be a litterbug’ sign in the classroom. In the attribution
group, the class had a sign saying ‘We are Anderson’s Litter-Conscious Class’
and they were praised for not littering etc. Modification of behaviour was
measured by seeing whether an assignment was thrown away after it was read,
this was compared to pre-test percentages. Those in the attribution group were
more likely to throw their rubbish away immediately following the study, as
well as two weeks post-study, compared to both the persuasion and control groups.
We want people to attribute themselves with the positive behaviour of saving
and therefore framed the challenge using ‘Saver’ to emphasise the implication
of saving for their identity rather than focusing on the action of ‘saving’.
Current and Future
Selves
Within the project, we emphasised that saving need not be
as much of a chore and saving little amounts is better than not saving anything
at all. The poster exemplifies this through 3 situations. The first depicts
someone who currently spends all their money (a ‘spendaholic’), and leaves
their ‘future self’ in a bad position, with no savings whatsoever.
The second scenario depicts someone who is currently frugal, but unhappy and their ‘future self’ is ultimately financially better off, but they have memories of unhappy times during their frugal saving stage.
From research we know that Millennials like to spend more money on socialising and creating memories (SPCE, 2017), and would therefore not enjoy the concept of being too frugal in the present just to have a future financial benefit. Our stick people were designed to make future selves and the benefits of saving more salient. A large barrier to saving-behaviour is temporal discounting, which is “the tendency to discount the value of future gains” (Joshi & Fast, 2015, pp. 432). People will often choose to receive a smaller reward immediately than a larger reward in the future. Joshi and Fast (2015) found that temporal discounting is partly caused by people feeling disconnected from their future selves. Additionally, Ersner-Hershfield, Garton, Ballard, Samanez-Larkin, and Knutson (2009) found that regardless of age, the amount of assets one has correlates positively with the amount one feels similar to future self. In a lab, participants who were shown what they may look like when they’re old, allocated more money into a hypothetical savings account (Hershfield et al., 2011). Also, Hershfield (2011) suggests that one can improve future self-continuity by manipulating future-self vividness, similarity and positivity.
Thus, we present a more ideal scenario involving our ‘Savvy Saver Challenge’. This scenario shows a happy ‘current self’ saving little amounts, but not being too frugal that they are missing out in the present. One’s ‘future self’ is shown to also be happy as they are financially better off for saving, but also happy when looking back at the fact they didn’t have to ‘starve to save’. Our stick people increase future-self vividness by pairing a future and current self, and positivity by showing that with the ‘Savvy Saver Challenge’, both their selves can be happy.
The second scenario depicts someone who is currently frugal, but unhappy and their ‘future self’ is ultimately financially better off, but they have memories of unhappy times during their frugal saving stage.
From research we know that Millennials like to spend more money on socialising and creating memories (SPCE, 2017), and would therefore not enjoy the concept of being too frugal in the present just to have a future financial benefit. Our stick people were designed to make future selves and the benefits of saving more salient. A large barrier to saving-behaviour is temporal discounting, which is “the tendency to discount the value of future gains” (Joshi & Fast, 2015, pp. 432). People will often choose to receive a smaller reward immediately than a larger reward in the future. Joshi and Fast (2015) found that temporal discounting is partly caused by people feeling disconnected from their future selves. Additionally, Ersner-Hershfield, Garton, Ballard, Samanez-Larkin, and Knutson (2009) found that regardless of age, the amount of assets one has correlates positively with the amount one feels similar to future self. In a lab, participants who were shown what they may look like when they’re old, allocated more money into a hypothetical savings account (Hershfield et al., 2011). Also, Hershfield (2011) suggests that one can improve future self-continuity by manipulating future-self vividness, similarity and positivity.
Thus, we present a more ideal scenario involving our ‘Savvy Saver Challenge’. This scenario shows a happy ‘current self’ saving little amounts, but not being too frugal that they are missing out in the present. One’s ‘future self’ is shown to also be happy as they are financially better off for saving, but also happy when looking back at the fact they didn’t have to ‘starve to save’. Our stick people increase future-self vividness by pairing a future and current self, and positivity by showing that with the ‘Savvy Saver Challenge’, both their selves can be happy.
Decision Tree – Interaction & Tailoring
Our decision
tree aims to increase participation in the challenge by using interaction and
tailoring. In a meta-analysis of health interventions, interactivity and
tailoring were factors that were successful in increasing positive attitudes
towards the intervention and encouraging behaviour change (Fjeldsoe, Marshall, & Miller, 2009). Tailoring is a more
individualised version of targeting (Kreuter
and Wray, 2003). Targeting aims information at one demographic: we targeted
students at Warwick using millennial colloquialism. Tailoring involves
specifying information to a person’s unique traits; our trees differentiate
between students that use cash or card, and students that have the motivation
to buy a piggy bank. Tailoring increases the personal relevance of information
to the viewer, which increases their engagement with the material (Noar, Benac, & Harris, 2007).
Interactivity is the ability of the viewer to change their outcome
(Steinemann, 2015). It is one of the fundamental templates of advertising,
which Goldenberg, Mazursky and Solomon (1999) have found increases the rating
of adverts. Steinemann (2015) have found that information presented in an
interactive way (controlling the speed of communication) increases hypothetical
donations. Therefore we think that interactive decision trees, where the viewer
finds their own outcome should be more interesting for our students.
Implementation
Intentions
The challenge itself is based
on Gollwitzer’s (1999) implementation intentions. Our students have a goal
intention: save £1 every day; but their implementation intention will specify
when, where and how the goal will be fulfilled e.g. when I hang my coat up I
will put £1 into the piggy bank next to it. We provided people with a chart to
keep track of whether they had saved each day.
We encouraged them to put their
savings pot (and provided the internet savers with a logo to print out) in a
spot that fitted into their routine (i.e. by their alarm clock so they would
save every time they set the clock). We recommended that they specify a time
and place to save each day, and use the item to remind them to fit it into
their routine.
We also sent
out a Facebook reminder each day at a varied interval. We used a varied
interval schedule as our savers were likely saving at different times of the
day. Gollwitzer (1999) reports that a single implementation intention can last
up to 3 weeks, but we wanted to keep their intention fresh in their mind. When
participants in Seehausen, Bayer and Gollwitzer’s study (as cited by
Gollwitzer, 1999) were told that they didn’t need to complete their goal, the
effect of the implementation intention disappeared in 48 hours. As our savers
weren’t saving for anything in particular, and were told they did not have to
save on the weekend, there was a danger that they may care less about achieving
the challenge as time goes on, so the reminders were also to strengthen the
concept of the goal.
Measuring
To measure how many people the challenged reached, we set
up a Facebook group page which people could like and access more information regarding
the challenge. Overall, 23 individuals liked our page, thus committing to the
challenge publically. According to Cialdini and Trost (1998) people have a
strong need to enhance their self-concepts by behaving consistently with their
actions, and public commitments increase compliance more than private
commitments.
References
Brown, J., Apostolova, V., Barton, C., Bolton, P., Noel,
D., Harari, D…. & Powell, A. (2017). Millennials.
House of Commons Library Briefing Paper CBP7946.
Bryan, C. J., Monin, B., & Adams, G. S. (2012). When
Cheating Would Make You a Cheater: Implicating the Self Prevents Unethical
Behaviour. Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 142(4), 1001-1005.
Cialdini, R. B., & Trost, M. R. (1998). Social
Influence: social norms, conformity, and compliance. In The Handbook of Social Psychology, 4th ed. Gilbert, D.
T., Fiske, S.T., & Lindzey, G. Vol. 2, 151–92. Boston: McGraw-Hill.
Corlett, A. (2017). Resolution
Foundation Intergeneration Commission Report. ‘As Time Goes By: Shifting
incomes and inequality between and within generations’. Online resource
retrieved from
https://www.resolutionfoundation.org/app/uploads/2017/02/IC-intra-gen.pdf on 26/04/18.
https://www.resolutionfoundation.org/app/uploads/2017/02/IC-intra-gen.pdf on 26/04/18.
Ersner-Hershfield,
H., Garton, M. T., Ballard, K., Samanez-Larkin, G. R., & Knutson, B.
(2009). Don’t stop thinking about tomorrow: Individual differences in future
self-continuity account for saving. Judgment and Decision Making, 4, 280-286
Fjeldsoe, B.
S., Marshall, A. L., & Miller, Y. D. (2009). Behavior change interventions
delivered by mobile telephone short-message service. American Journal of Preventive
Medicine, 36,165-173.
Fleming, J. H. (2017). Millennials Most Likely to Shop for Fun, on Impulse. Gallup Business
Journal: Online Resource retrieved from http://news.gallup.com/businessjournal/191852/millennials-likely-shop-fun-impulse.aspx
on 20/04/18.
Goldenberg, J., Mazursky, D.,
& Solomon, S. (1999). The fundamental templates of quality ads. Marketing
Science, 18, 333-351.
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation
intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist,
54, 493-503.
Halpern, D. (2016). Inside the nudge
unit: How small changes can make a big difference. London: Random House.
Hershfield, H. E. (2011). Future
self‐continuity: How conceptions of the future self-transform intertemporal
choice. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1235, 30-43.
Hershfield, H. E., Goldstein, D. G., Sharpe, W.
F., Fox, J., Yeykelis, L., Carstensen, L. L., & Bailenson, J. N. (2011).
Increasing saving behavior through age-progressed renderings of the future
self. Journal of Marketing Research, 48, S23-S37.
Joshi, P. D., & Fast, N.
J. (2013). Power and reduced temporal discounting. Psychological
Science, 24, 432-438.
Kreuter, M.
W., & Wray, R. J. (2003). Tailored and targeted health communication:
strategies for enhancing information relevance. American Journal of
Health Behavior, 27, S227-S232.
Miller, R. L., Brickman, P., & Bolen, D. (1975).
Attribution versus persuasion as a means of modifying behaviour. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 31(3), 430-441.
Mintel. (2016). Baby
Food and Drink – UK – April 2016. Mintel International Group Limited.
Nielsen. (2015). Global
Sustainability Report: ‘The
Sustainability Imperative, New Insights on Consumer Expectations’. Online
Resource retrieved from http://www.nielsen.com/content/dam/nielsenglobal/co/docs/Reports/2015/global-sustainability-report.pdf
on 25/02/18.
Noar, S. M.,
Benac, C. N., & Harris, M. S. (2007). Does tailoring matter? Meta-analytic
review of tailored print health behavior change interventions. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 673-693.
Office for National Statistics. (2017). Housing Affordability in England and Wales:
2016. Online Resource retrieved from https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/housing/bulletins/housingaffordabilityinenglandandwales/1997to2016
on 20/04/18.
Oxford Dictionary. (2018). Millennial [Definition]. Online
resource retrieved from https://en.oxforddictionaries.com/definition/millennial
on 02/04/18 on 25/04/18.
SPCE. (2017). Student
Spending Index. SPCE Labs. Online Resource retrieved from https://www.liveinspce.com/spce-labs
on 01/04/18.
Steinemann, S. T., Mekler, E.
D., & Opwis, K. (2015). Increasing donating behavior through a game for
change: The role of interactivity and appreciation. Proceedings of the 2015
Annual Symposium on Computer-Human Interaction in Play, ACM, 319-329.
US Census Bureau. (2015). ACS Demographic and Housing Estimates 2011-2015 American Community Survey 5-Year Estimates.
Retrieved from https://www.census.gov/acs/www/data/data-tables-and-tools/data-profiles/2015/
on 01/04/18.
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.