We’ve heard about their
stories, but how many of us have helped?
According
to Shacknove (1985), a refugee is someone who would be at risk if they were to
return to their home country. The European refugee crisis began in 2015 where
it became a struggle for many countries, including the UK, to cope with the
increase in numbers (“Migrant crisis”, 2016). Whilst the government has an obligation
to support these refugees and their families, there are also numerous
charities which aim to help with resettling, providing resources and
integrating refugees into their new homes, lives and communities.
Many
of these charities rely on donations and help from the general public which
raises the question, how many of us have helped?
Step 1: Identifying the problem at Warwick
We
surveyed 81 students and found that whilst 97.5% of people believed refugees
should be supported, only 31% of people had previously made an effort to help.
Evidently, this discrepancy is substantial, so we decided that something needed
to be done to change this. According to the theory of planned behaviour (Azjen,
1991), having an attitude towards something is not enough to predict behaviours
towards it. Individuals require self-efficacy in order to believe that they
they are capable of taking action, and that this action will be effective in
achieving what it intends to. Although self-efficacy is important, increasing the availability of action must also be considered. Behaviours can be made more
available by showing others that it’s possible and increasing their perceived
behavioural control.
Being
students ourselves, who are all too familiar with the time consuming nature of
university life, we recognised it’s not that students don’t want to help (we’re
good people at heart!), it’s the struggle in trying to balance academic life
alongside making time to get involved with the causes they care about. Our
survey found 92.6% of students would be willing to help in ways which would not
require a regular time commitment (as seen in Figure 1). Of these students, 53%
stated they would be like to be contacted with information regarding ways they
can get involved to help the local refugees. This valuable insight allowed us
to come up with a plan to change the behaviour of those 53% so they would
actively make the difference they want to make with the local refugees in
Coventry.
Figure 1. A pie chart showing how many students would be willing to
help without making a time commitment.
Coventry
has been in the news for its aid in resettling many Syrian refugees in the UK
(“Coventry City Council”, 2016). We found the local Coventry Migrant and
Refugee Centre located in the heart of Coventry offers many services targeting
housing, employment, and mental wellbeing issues for refugees. Therefore, we
decided a great way for students to get involved in helping local refugees
would be to contribute to this centre, whether this be simply visiting and
interacting with the refugees, or stopping by to deliver donations.
Step 2: Arranging for visits and donations from students
Research
has established ‘just asking’ gets you what you want. For example, Clark and
Hatfield (1989) found that 50% of people would be willing to go on a date with
a stranger who simply made the nerve-racking move of asking them. Using this principle, we
asked the 53% of students who were willing to be contacted in the future
whether they would like to join us on a visit to the local refugee centre. We
were also able to utilise the foot-in-the-door technique (Freedman and Fraser,
1966), which sees an individual being more willing to complete a larger
request, such as putting up a safe driving poster, if they have already
completed a smaller task for you, such as signing a petition about safe
driving. In this case, participants completed a small task by filling in the
questionnaire, which subsequently made them feel more committed when they were
asked to donate or visit the centre at a later date.
We
arranged a date to visit the centre and encouraged individuals to bring
donations of food and toiletries with them. On the day, a group of students
arrived with generous bags of donations, these were gratefully received by the
staff at the centre (see Figure 2-3).
Figure 2. An image of us and a staff member at the centre
with some of our donations.
Figure 3. An image of some of the
students who signed up to visit the centre with us.
From
asking the students about their experience, we found many expressed that they
were surprised at how easy it was to donate and that they would be willing to
do it again! From this we realised, we were on our way to positively changing
the behaviour of these students. This is due to the principles of commitment
and consistency discussed by Cialdini (2007) . One study found that voter turnout increased in the USA if
people were called beforehand and asked whether or not they would vote. The
individuals in the study had already committed themselves to voting and
therefore were more likely to actually vote in order to stay consistent
(Greenwald, 1987). Similarly, once the students expressed an interest in
supporting refugees, they felt committed when we later asked them to visit and
donate towards the centre. After this visit, the students had successfully
incorporated the characteristic of being a prosocial person who supports
refugees into their self-image.
Step 3: Getting bigger businesses involved
Having
gained a new found love of just asking, we targeted local businesses to ask if
they would be willing to put some items aside to be donated to the centre.
Importantly, we also used flattery as research has shown flattery often gets
you what you want (Pratkanis & Abott, 2011). This was done by starting the
phone calls with statements such as “We know you’re a generous company that is
always helping out the local community”. By first complimenting them, it was
more likely they would comply with our subsequent request. This technique is
supported by Kraut (1973) who found labelling individuals as helpful, made it
more likely they would provide a more generous contribution to a cause. This
turned out to be a success as we were able to gather a variety of items to
donate to the centre! There were particular items that the refugee centre did
not have to facilities to accept, so we reached out to another charity based in
Coventry called Carriers of Hope who are also involved in helping out refugees.
Figure 4 shows some of the stationery, homeware and toys donated by a Tiger
store.
Figure 4. Some of our generous donations from businesses in Coventry
and Leamington Spa.
Step 4: Collecting monetary donations
For
those who were unable to visit the centre, we offered the the opportunity to
donate money through our JustGiving page. This employed the principle of just
asking as we explicitly questioned whether or not they would like to make a
donation. Again, the foot-in-the-door technique was used as individuals would
have been more willing to complete a larger request, such as making a monetary
donation, if they had already completed a smaller task for us, such as filling
out our questionnaire (Freedman and Fraser, 1966). It came as no surprise that
people were willing to help and were likely to make a donation. This brings us
on to the fact that most of our donations were of £10 which can be explained by
the anchoring effect. As our first few donations were of £10 each, this created
an anchor which people based their own decision off.
Step 5: Attracting the wider student population
In
order to target the wider student population, and not limit ourselves to our
survey respondents, we created a poster to inform people of ways they could
help refugees. The image of children overlaying the background was used to
evoke empathy and make people think of themselves as a child, as individuals
are more likely to help when placing themselves in the situation of another
person (Archer, Foushee, Davis, & Aderman, 1979).We used a QR code which
contained more in-depth information as research demonstrates that practical
users, such as those who want to find out more information about the
advertisement, are more likely to scan QR codes (Ozkaya, Ozkaya, Roxas, Bryant,
& Whitson (2015).
The
poster used both System 1 and System 2 thinking (Kahneman, 2011). The poster
was kept very minimalistic to ensure that the necessary information was
conveyed with ease, this enabled us to employ System 1 thinking, which is
automatic and requires little attention. As we were targeting people who wanted
to help, this engaged them in System 2 thinking and made them more likely to
obtain further information about the cause, leaving them thinking about the
message. The posters were placed in a range of places, including the inside of
lifts, common rooms, the library, corridors, and outdoor areas on campus (see
Figure 5). The majority of the text featured on the poster directly addressed
the audience and used rhetorical questions. The use of rhetoric has been found
to be persuasive when a strong argument is being presented, such as ours when
the QR code is scanned (Petty, Cacioppo, & Heesacker, 1981; Tom and Eves,
1999). The directness of the posters inclines the audience to address the
poster (Hyland, 2008).
Additionally,
the posters made the refugee crisis more available in the minds of students.
Tversky and Kahneman (1973) proposed the availability heuristic, whereby easy
retrieval of something increases its importance in an individual’s mind. The
posters encourage students to think about the issue, making it seem more
important to them. This would subsequently make the students more inclined to
think about the issue and potentially make efforts to help in the future,
therefore this was an advantage for the long-term.
Figure 5. Images of our poster in various locations around campus.
A summary of the steps we took to elicit behaviour change:
As
of now we have gained enough donations to fund 10 food parcels with enough
provisions for 50 meals! However, we can always do more, so please feel free to
donate to our Just Giving page.
Anything
will help, but should we say 17 cents? (Santos, Leve, & Pratkanis, 1994)
References
Archer,
R. L., Foushee, H. C., Davis, M. H., & Aderman, D. (1979). Emotional
empathy in a courtroom simulation: A person-situation interaction. Journal
of Applied Social Psychology, 9, 275-291.
Ajzen,
I. ( 1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and
Human Decision Processes, 50, 179-211.
Burnkrant,
R. E., & Howard, D. J. (1984). Effects of the use of introductory
rhetorical questions versus statements on information processing. Journal of
Personality and social psychology, 47, 1218.
Cialdini,
R. B. (2007). Influence: The psychology of persuasion. New York:
Collins.
Clark,
R. D., & Hatfield, E. (1989). Gender differences in receptivity to sexual
offers. Journal of Psychology & Human Sexuality, 2, 39-55.
Coventry
city council takes most Syrian refugees. (2016, May 27). BBC. Retrieved
from http://www.bbc.co.uk
Freedman,
J. L., & Fraser, S. C. (1966). Compliance without pressure: The
foot-in-the-door technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4,
195-202.
Greenwald,
A. G., Carnot, C. G., Beach, R., & Young, B. (1987). Increasing voting
behavior by asking people if they expect to vote. Journal of Applied
Psychology, 72, 315-318.
Hyland,
K. (2008). Persuasion, interaction and the construction of knowledge:
Representing self and others in research writing. International Journal of
English Studies, 8, 1-23.
Khaneman,
D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
Migrant
crisis: Migration to Europe explained in seven charts. (2016, March 4). BBC.
Retrieved from http://www.bbc.co.uk/
Tom,
G., & Eves, A. (1999). The use of rhetorical devices in advertising. Journal
of Advertising Research, 39, 39-44.
Ozkaya,
E., Ozkaya, H. E., Roxas, J., Bryant, F., & Whitson, D. (2015). Factors
affecting consumer usage on QR codes. Journal of Direct, Data and Digital
Marketing Practice, 16, 209-224.
Petty,
R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Heesacker, M. (1981). Effects of rhetorical
questions on persuasion: A cognitive response analysis. Journal of
personality and social psychology, 40, 432.
Pratkanis,
A. R., & Abbott, C. J. (2011). Flattery and compliance with a direct
request: Towards a theory of toady influence. Unpublished manuscript.
University of California, Santa Cruz.
Santos,
M. D., Leve, C., & Pratkanis, A. R. (1994). Hey buddy, can you spare
seventeen cents? Mindful persuasion and the pique technique. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 24, 755-764.
Shacknove,
A. E. (1985). Who is a Refugee?. Ethics, 95, 274-284.
Spence,
D. (2012). Flattery will get you everywhere. British Medical Journal, 345,
51.
Aisha Zahid, Haneefah Pervez, Ifra Ali
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.