This image was published by
Public Health England (PHE) as part of a series of anti-smoking advertisements. It does not set out to sugar-coat the effects of smoking
however it presents the health risks of smoking in a raw and graphic manner, comparing it to a process similar to rotting. This is an example of shock advertising which is regarded
as an appeal that purposely shocks and offends its audience. Many of us will
have come across an advert like the one above yet a key question to consider is
how effective such strategies really are. When a smoker sees an image like
this, he/she is likely to experience a sentiment of shock and even disgust.
However to what extent is the use of shocking images effective in actually
changing one’s behaviour and in this case, encouraging one to quit smoking?
This technique is
demonstrated in a study by Dahl, Frankenberger and Machanda (2003) who
investigated the effectiveness of shock advertising used in HIV/AIDS prevention,
in comparison to advertisements involving fear and information. The 'shock' advertisement featured a headline that read “Don’t’ be a F-ing Idiot” and featured a nude couple in an intimate embrace.The 'fear' advertisement showed a
driver’s license with the expiration date circled in red and the headline “If
you get the AIDS virus now, you and your license could expire at the same time”.The 'information' advertisement showed the AIDS acronym with the
words “Acquired Immunodeficiency Syndrome”. All three advertisements featured
the tagline “Use a Condom”. There was also a control condition where the “Use a Condom” poster was absent.
Subjects were randomly
assigned to one of the four conditions (shock, fear, information or control) and
subjects in each condition were left alone in a room with the stimulus
poster and four decoy posters visible. Following this, subjects were directed to a table showing several items that were said to no longer be wanted. There were a total of eight items,
three of which were used to measure the dependent variable. These three items
were (1) condom knowledge from Durex (2) AIDS ribbons (3) a business card with
the HIV/AIDS hotline from a local medical clinic. Subjects were told that they could keep any items that they wanted. The experimenter
left the room and returned two minutes later. Once the subjects left the
room, the experimenter counted the items and made a note of which had been
picked up.
Similar positive effects were found for the shock and fear conditions. According to Dahl, Frankenberger and Machanda (2003), these appeals operate in fundamentally different ways. Shock appeals are thought to be effective in producing message-consistent behaviour because they violate norms which surprises subjects and produces additional cognitive processing. Fear appeals however are thought to be effective because they tap into more affective responses from an audience.
Advertisement agencies as well as public policy makers should consider the use of shocking and fearful content in campaigns because although we might not always want to see the gruesome effects that smoking can have on our bodies, sometimes shock advertisements are just what we need to make us think twice before lighting up our next cigarette.
Dahl,
D.W., Frankenberger, K.D., & Machanda, R.V. (2003). Does it pay to shock?
Reactions to shocking and nonshocking advertising content among university
students. Journal of Advertising
Research, 43, 268-280.
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.