Trust, Emotion, Threats
and Mistakes: An Observation of Negotiation Knowledge in Ordinary, Unfamiliar
People
By Jessica Varghese & Azhar Munim
Background
Inspired by the negotiation exercises performed during
lectures, this blog post provides an overview of a collaborative task designed
to test multiple negotiation skills taught in the PS379 module.
Learning about how to successfully negotiate during
these lectures was incredibly useful, but not everyone has had the privilege to
learn and apply these skills. This results in many people making common
negotiation mistakes due to underestimating the value of each of their actions.
So, the main aim of conducting this task was to observe
how ordinary people would react to negotiation challenges. We observed the
mistakes they made, but also which strategies they would unknowingly apply
without any prior knowledge of negotiation and influence.
Introduction
What happens when every person in the room has part of
the truth — but no one is willing to give it away for free?
In our negotiation task, we designed and ran a
negotiation-based role-playing experience centred on the aftermath of an
international artifact heist. Five players, each holding a unique piece of the
puzzle, had to work with (or against) each other to recover the stolen item.
However, each player was a suspect, with a hidden core goal they each wished to
achieve.
The freedom of action, combined with the novel
situation, revealed how people actually behaved in complex negotiations – but
also how quickly negotiations spiral when alliances break down, threats are made,
and all trust fizzles away.
This blog unpacks the key decisions, negotiation
strategies and group dynamics that shaped the game. We have drawn on research about
negotiation and influence to interpret the players’ choices and explain each
members potential thought processes. This allows us to assess whether our
predictions of ‘ordinary behaviour’ were correct, or if negotiations are truly
as unpredictable as human nature...
The Scenario:
"Warwick, 2096.
Once a university. Now an archive of art, memory and power.
After the War of Wick reshaped the world, the Faculty of Arts building was rebuilt
as a museum – a resting place for disputed histories.
Somewhere beneath the marble and glass, the Wick Blade was locked away.
An artifact with a dangerous past. A blade passed between
powerful hands, just before everything fell apart.
Last night, it disappeared.
No noise. No trace. Just silence.
Now, five individuals have been summoned behind closed
doors.
A historian. A curator. A dealer. An agent. A forger.
Each had something to offer – and something to hide.
The goal: recover the dagger, rewrite the story, protect what matters most".
Recruitment
Five individuals willing to take part in a “negotiation
task” were brought into a room together, accompanied by us. Each participant
was made aware of the scenario above, and was randomly assigned one of the
following roles, all with private objectives and leverage.
Below is a brief description of each of the five roles,
alongside the description cards provided to the respective player.
(On request of the negotiators, we have kept their
real identities private, only referring to their roles in this blog)
Character |
Identity |
Core
Goal |
Holds… |
The Forger |
Black-market artist who forged a
replica |
Immunity from prosecution |
Details of the fake dagger |
Interpol Agent |
Law enforcer looking for a public
arrest |
Reputation & justice |
Surveillance image of the thief |
Museum Curator |
Insider facing exposure |
Self-preservation |
Timeline of the theft + access
codes |
Black Market Dealer |
Fixer with underground links |
Crypto payment or revenge |
Name of the buyer |
Historian |
Academic expert in artifacts |
Exclusive publishing rights |
Political origin of the dagger |
Theoretical Lens
We didn’t brief players on negotiation concepts
beforehand. Instead, we designed the scenario to test key ideas from
negotiation and influence theory, including:
Distributive negotiations
Ø Positional
bargaining (Patton, 2015): negotiating from their initial positions and demands
whilst refusing to change perspectives as the value of information shifts
Ø Viewing
the negotiation as a ‘win or lose’ situation
Ø Attempting
to individually maximise their gains from an assumed ‘fixed sum’
Integrative negotiations
Ø Solving
problems together to benefit multiple negotiators – as opposed to haggling for control
or personal ideals (Fisher & Ury, 2012)
Ø Being
clear about their issues and goals to trade and negotiate with others – logrolling
Ø “Expanding
the pie” - exploring options to increase the total negotiation benefits (Benetti
et al., 2021)
BATNA – Best Alternatives to a Negotiated Agreement
Ø If
participants can recognise the consequences of ‘walking away’ from a
negotiation (Fisher, 1991)
Ø Knowing
when to back out of a negotiation – such as if the alternative is better than
its likely outcome (Wang & Zionts, 2008)
Ø Identifying
the BATNAs of everyone involved, and how much each negotiator is willing to
negotiate around it – having a costly BATNA demonstrates bargaining weakness (McKibben,
2020)
Ø Keeping negotiation offers within the ZOPA
ZOPA – Zone of Possible Agreement
Ø The gap between the BATNAs in a negotiation
Ø Agreements
preferred by the involved groups (Cavatorta et al., 2025)
Ø The
existence of multiple BATNAs at once means there is not one single common ZOPA
– negotiators will need to decide who to appease, and who to disregard
3D negotiation – “Tactics, Deal Design, Setup” (Lax
& Sebenius, 2006)
Ø Tactics:
Understanding the nature of distributive and integrative negotiations
Ø Deal
Design: Expanding the outlines of the deal, increasing its value
Ø Setup: Approaching the negotiation to create the most promising environment to achieve your goals – such as asserting control or forming alliances
Influence and Coercion
Ø Judging
if participants will resort to verbal, relational, or emotional threats to
leverage control in the negotiations
Ø Acknowledging
that failed threats will create scepticism in their confidence (Kim, 2022)
Ø Whether
roles which depend on sabotaging others for success (Interpol Officer, Black Market
Dealer), which incorporate a ‘madman strategy’, unpredictably acting against their
own interests to undermine their opposition (Boussios, 2021)
Managing Difficult Conversations
Ø Conflict
of interests can cause feelings of stress, anxiety and anger – potentially impairing
rational thinking, animosity, and exclusions in the group (Ellis, 2020)
Ø Participants who disregard values of trust and resort to personal ego attacks are likely to fail in negotiations
Ø
Participants who disregard values of trust and
resort to personal ego attacks are likely to fail in negotiations
Ø Valuing the importance of listening to minimise resentment and misinformation, while encouraging the sharing of thoughts and opinions (Weinstein et al., 2022)
These negotiation frameworks helped us understand what potential decisions would be made based on theory, and in turn, how real negotiation may differ from “ideal” negotiation.
Our
Predictions of Behaviour
The unpredictable nature of human behaviour and the
open-ended negotiation task make it difficult to predict a set outcome or a specific sequence of events.
However, from our knowledge of negotiation strategies
and their common pitfalls, we can anticipate specific behaviours we expect to see
from a group of ordinary people, unfamiliar with negotiation and influence.
BATNAs, ZOPAs,
and First Offers
We expect negotiators to overlook the importance of
their BATNA and ZOPA, leading to poor first offers.
We suspect these first offers to be either overly generous, giving up too much
negotiating ground, or overly aggressive, causing immediate rejection.
On the other hand, we similarly suspect that poorly managed counteroffers will
expose too much information, weakening their position. In turn, this may make a
previously ineffective first offer into a useful one.
Distributive vs
Integrative Negotiations
We predict that negotiators will likely approach the
task as distributive at first, aiming to maximise their personal gain whilst
withholding personal information.
However, as discussions progress and shared interests between negotiators
emerge, we expect a shift towards integrative negotiation, leading to more
efficient agreements in retrieving the artifact (Brett & Thompson, 2016).
We additionally suspect that negotiators who have
singled themselves out as weak by exposing too much personal information will
be excluded from integrative negotiation agreements.
Threats, Trust,
and Ego
We anticipate that emotions, threats and ego will be
key factors in the negotiation outcome.
Those who use emotional intelligence to build alliances
are likely to succeed, while those who provoke anger or engage in personal
attacks will lose credibility, leverage, and disrupt the group’s negotiations
(Steinel et al., 2008).
We predict that threats will specifically play an
important role, with individuals who yield under pressure ending up with poorer
negotiation results.
What
Happened in the Game?
The negotiation session unfolded organically, without any
formal structure, yet numerous distinct phases and turning points emerged.
These events accurately mirrored fundamental theories and psychological
phenomena that we explored throughout PS379, and ultimately, they impacted the
distribution of power and the final outcome.
In this section, we will go over
each key turning point sequentially. For each moment, we will include quotes
from our participants to illustrate their decision making, as well as a thorough
analysis based on the theories that we learnt in PS370 and wider literature. By
doing this, we will be able to critically assess what happened, why
participants behaved the way they did, and how theory can explain/fail to
explain real negotiation dynamics.
Turning Point 1: Early Anchoring
and Loss of Power
The Interpol Agent assertively
began the negotiation by demanding transparency from all parties. The Museum
Curator, feeling vulnerable and exposed, quickly gave in and disclosed
sensitive information such as access logs and security information to prove
that she wasn’t involved in the theft.
“I felt like if I didn’t
move quickly, I was going to be the scapegoat. I had to give them something, even if it wasn’t much at all.” - Museum Curator
Analysis:
This early concession
demonstrates the risks of anchoring bias and premature information disclosure (Hills,
2024, Lecture 3). The Curator’s decision to reveal crucial information without
gaining anything in return set a weak anchor that reduced her negotiation
power.
According to distributive
negotiation theory, premature disclosures are strategic errors that allow the
opponents to shape the negotiation space in their favour (Hills, 2024, Lecture
3). Additionally, the Curator had poor awareness of their BATNA, failing to
recognise alternative strategies such as forming defensive alliances or
withholding information in order to maintain negotiating leverage (McKibben,
2013).
Outcome: As a result of this
premature disclosure, the Curator lost essential negotiating leverage early on.
She was viewed as vulnerable by the other participants, making her a prime
target for pressure tactics and emotional coercion in subsequent interactions.
Turning Point 2: Breakdown of
Trust and Missed Integrative Deal
Shortly later, the Forger
approached the Historian and proposed an integrative exchange: immunity
protection in exchange for exclusive rights to publish the findings regarding
the origin of the dagger. Although this arrangement could have been mutually beneficial
and created value for both parties, the Historian declined the Forger’s offer
due to distrust.
“It sounded like a fair deal, but
it didn’t feel like the right time. I wasn’t sure if I could trust her.” -
Historian
Analysis: This missed opportunity
emphasises how fragile integrative negotiation efforts can be when relational
trust is lacking (Hills, 2024, Lecture 4). According to negotiation theory,
logrolling, exchanging concessions across issues of different value, is only
effective when there is sufficient trust to make those trades feel secure (Lecture
4; Kuwabara, 2011; Moran & Ritov, 2002). Additionally, Curhan et al. (2006)
argue that negotiators value subjective feelings about fairness and trust, as much
as, if not more than, objective outcomes, which explains why the historian
declined this rational offer.
Outcome: The breakdown of trust
in this early exchange created an atmosphere of growing suspicion among the
participants. Consequently, future collaboration opportunities became more difficult
to identify and pursue, steering the negotiation environment towards
defensiveness and competition.
Turning Point 3: Information
Hoarding and Defensive Bargaining
Following the early betrayals and
missed deals, the negotiating environment became increasingly defensive.
Participants resorted to information hoarding, making vague proposals and hints, but refusing to reveal specifics unless directly reciprocated.
“Everyone kept dropping hints,
but no one was willing to share anything unless they had something to gain from
it” – Museum Curator.
This transition marks a shift
from integrative to distributive bargaining, prompted by the deterioration of
trust amongst the participants (Hills, 2024, Lecture 4). When negotiators perceive
trust as broken, they tend to move away from problem-solving and instead treat
every piece of information as a bargaining chip to be protected. Steinel et al.
(2010) found that negotiators strategically withhold information under conditions
of low trust and adopt more defensive and competitive behaviours to protect
their own positions. Instead of expanding the pie through collaboration, the participants
began to view the negotiation as a zero-sum struggle, where any disclosure of
information was viewed as a potential liability.
This defensive mindset aligns
with difficult conversations theory (Hills, 2024, Lecture 5), which explains
how emotional defensiveness and fear of exploitation can erode the foundations
of cooperative behaviour, replacing open conversation with self-defensive behaviour.
Outcome: After open information
sharing broke down, the negotiation shifted into a zero-sum struggle.
Participants became increasingly focused on protecting their own interests,
which made it more difficult to generate joint value or explore creative agreements.
Turning Point 4: Emotional
Coercion and Blackmail
As the negotiation intensified,
the Black Market Dealer used an emotionally charged threat – he exposed private
texts indicating the Curator’s involvement in the theft. Though the threat was
never actually executed, the threat alone was enough to induce compliance.
“I didn’t actually have to follow
through. I just wanted to show him that I had the power, that’s all.” – Black Market
Dealer
Analysis: The Dealer’s implied
threat is consistent with major principles of coercive influence and blackmail
dynamics in negotiation theory (Hills, 2024, Lecture 10). In such situations,
behaviour change is driven by the perception of a credible threat rather than
the execution of the explicit threat (Lecture 10; Sinaceur & Neale, 2005).
The Curator’s response demonstrates how identity-based threats, such as
reputational risks, can override rational decision-making and shift the focus
to emotional self-protection (Klaassen et al., 2021).
Outcome: The Dealer’s coercive tactics not only forced
critical concessions from the Curator but also normalised the use of threats as
a legitimate bargaining tool within the group. The rise in emotional pressure
further eroded trust and destabilised trust and strategic cooperation among all
participants.
Turning Point 5: Identity Crisis
and Competitive Death Spiral
As the participants became more
emotionally reactive, mainly the Curator and Interpol Agent, the negotiation
deteriorated into a competitive death spiral. Accusations escalated,
collaboration broke down, and irrational behaviour became increasingly more common.
“After a certain point, I
stopped trying to win. I was just trying to save face.” – Curator
Analysis: This spiral depicts the
dynamics of identity threat and emotional escalation described in the difficult
conversations lecture (Lecture 5; Kopelman et al., 2006) When negotiators feel
that their competence, integrity, or self-worth are being attacked, they shift
from problem-solving to defensive posturing, ultimately leading to negotiation
breakdown (Kopelman et al., 2006). In addition, Movius and Wilson (2011) show
that negotiators frequently misjudge and struggle to navigate emotional
dynamics, which can escalate conflict rather than resolving it. The emotional
deterioration observed here supports the notion that effective negotiation
depends as much on managing identity and emotions as on exchanging offers (Steinel
et al, 2008)
Outcome: As emotions escalated,
marginalised participants such as the Curator and Interpol Agent struggled to
effectively negotiate. Their responses became reactive rather than strategic,
leading them to lose influence and leaving them sidelined in the final stages of
the negotiation.
Turning Point 6: Coalition
Formation and Strategic Exclusion
As trust deteriorated, the
Forger, Dealer and Historian established a private strategic coalition. They
exchanged information discreetly and negotiated independently with the buyer of
the artifact and intentionally excluded the Interpol Agent and Curator from
critical discussions.
“I thought this whole thing was
about justice. Turns out, it was all about leverage to them.” – Interpol Agent
Analysis: This coalition demonstrates the core principles of 3D negotiation, particularly setup strategy and away-from-the-table moves (Lax & Sebenius, 2006; Lecture 6). Instead of competing at the negotiating table, the members of the coalition shaped the negotiation dynamics by off-table coordination and exclusion. According to Kuwabara (2011), strong relational cohesion enables subgroups to dominate negotiations, when broader trust breaks down. In addition to coalition formation, the sequencing and timing of discreet discussions strengthened their strategic advantage (Lax & Sebenius, 2006).
Outcome: The formation of the
private coalition significantly shifted the power balance. By pooling
information and isolating others, the coalition members were able to dictate
the final terms of the negotiation and fulfil their personal goals without opposition.
Turning Point 7: Failure of
Principled Strategy (Interpol Agent)
The Interpol Agent’s steadfast
commitment to justice and transparency eventually worked against him. His
failure to adapt strategically or establish alliances led to him being
marginalised and excluded from the final agreement.
“I kept believing that they would
do the right thing. But they never did.” – Interpol Agent
Analysis: This outcome emphasises
the strategic limitation of rigid principled negotiation approaches (Patton,
2015; Lecture 6). As emphasised in 3D negotiation theory (Lax & Sebenius,
2006), success in multi-party contexts requires adaptive engagement with
shifting coalitions and stakeholder interests. The Interpol Agent’s failure
shows that maintaining the moral high ground, without accompanying strategic
flexibility and relational management, is rarely enough to gain influence or
achieve favourable outcomes.
Outcome: The Interpol Agent’s unwavering commitment to
moral principles ultimately led to his strategic marginalisation. His
reluctance to adapt to the evolving alliance-based structure of the negotiation
left him excluded from critical agreements and left him without a voice in the
negotiation’s outcome.
Participant
Reflections
After the negotiation task was completed, participants were independently asked to reflect on their own behaviour and decision-making. We also presented them with statements related to trust, pressure, and tactics, and asked them to indicate their level of agreement.
Statement |
Agreement |
“Trust influenced my decisions.” |
4/5
ppts |
“I felt pressure to concede.” |
4/5
ppts |
“I used deception to achieve my
goals.” |
3/5
ppts |
“I would approach this
differently now.” |
5/5
ppts |
“I had no idea how to properly approach this scenario
at the beginning” |
5/5 ppts |
A few participants provided additional remarks to these statements, highlighting their changing thought processes as the negotiation began to spiral out of control, and grew much more defensive. These open-ended reflections were collected immediately after the task and gave us insight into each participant's reasoning and their emotional response.
The following quotes are examples of these reflections:
"I thought I could play it smart, but by halfway
through, I was just bluffing to not get caught.” – Interpol Agent
"I didn’t realise how quickly people
could just shift. I mean, I had started off cooperative, then stopped trusting
anyone to protect myself.” – Forger
Note that the quotes featured in the Turning Points section were also drawn from these post-game reflections.
Conclusion:
What the Outcomes Showed
Our negotiation task revealed how ‘ordinary’
negotiators instinctively applied key theories and strategies, highlighting the
importance of negotiation and influence education.
We successfully predicted many behaviours, such as the
lasting importance of a poor BATNA on leverage and confidence, seen with the
Museum Curator. We also correctly anticipated the significant role that threats
and emotions would play, with the Curator once again being vulnerable to implied
threats, with their distrust preventing early alliances. Emotional exposure
further weakened positions, with private alliances forming against the Interpol
Agent due to the explosive behaviour and loss of leverage.
However, we were surprised that negotiations began
somewhat integrative before shifting to a distributive manner over time. We had
predicted negotiations to start reluctantly, with the negotiators gradually
forming alliances. But we underestimated how initial interactions, driven by a
need for security and trust, encouraged a more cooperative early phase.
Overall, this blog has shown that those unfamiliar with
negotiation theory tend to rely on instinct and social cues, making them more
prone to mistakes and emotional influence. This contributes to and explains the
unpredictable nature of negotiations at all levels, from everyday disputes to
international diplomacy.
Thank you for reading our blog post – we hope it has demonstrated the importance
of studying negotiation and influence for success in everyday interactions.
References
Benetti,
S., Ogliastri, E., & Caputo, A. (2021). Distributive/integrative
negotiation strategies in cross-cultural contexts: a comparative study of the
USA and Italy. Journal of Management & Organization, 27(4),
786–808.
Boussios,
E. G. (2020). Hacking Back: Trump’s “Madman Theory” Approach to Cybersecurity. Journal
of Applied Security Research, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1080/19361610.2020.1832860
Brett, J.,
& Thompson, L. (2016). Negotiation. Organizational Behavior and Human
Decision Processes, 136(1), 68–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2016.06.003
Cavatorta,
E., Groom, B., & Sher, G. (2024). Revealing the zone of possible agreement
between parties in conflict: An application to Israeli-Palestinian peace
agreements. PNAS Nexus, 4(1). https://doi.org/10.1093/pnasnexus/pgae581
Curhan, J.
R., Elfenbein, H. A., & Xu, H. (2006). What do people value when they
negotiate? Mapping the domain of subjective value in negotiation. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 91(3), 493–512. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.493
Ellis, D.
G. (2020). Talking to the Enemy: Difficult Conversations and Ethnopolitical
Conflict. Negotiation and Conflict Management Research, 13(3),
183–196. https://doi.org/10.1111/ncmr.12187
Fisher,
R., & Urey, W. (1982). Getting to Yes: Negotiating Agreement without Giving
in. International Journal, 37(4), 649. https://doi.org/10.2307/40202101
Kim, J. Y.
(2021). Negotiation statements with promise and threat. Review of Economic
Design, 26(2), 149–164. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10058-021-00261-8
Klaassen,
F. H., Held, L., Figner, B., O’Reilly, J. X., Klumpers, F., de Voogd, L. D.,
& Roelofs, K. (2021). Defensive freezing and its relation to
approach–avoidance decision-making under threat. Scientific Reports, 11(1).
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-90968-z
Kopelman,
S., Rosette, A. S., & Thompson, L. (2006). The three faces of Eve:
Strategic displays of positive, negative, and neutral emotions in negotiations.
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 99(1),
81–101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2005.08.003
Kuwabara,
K. (2011). Cohesion, Cooperation, and the Value of Doing Things Together. American
Sociological Review, 76(4), 560–580. https://doi.org/10.1177/0003122411414825
Lax, D.
A., & Sebenius, J. K. (2006). 3-D negotiation: powerful tools to change the
game in your most important deals. Harvard Business School Press.
McKibben,
H. E. (2013). The Effects of Structures and Power on State Bargaining
Strategies. American Journal of Political Science, 57(2),
411–427. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5907.2012.00628.x
McKibben,
H. E. (2020). What do I get? How do states’ negotiation alternatives influence
the concessions they receive in multilateral negotiations? European Journal
of International Relations, 26(3), 135406612090687. https://doi.org/10.1177/1354066120906875
Moran, S.,
& Ritov, I. (2002). Initial perceptions in negotiations: evaluation and
response to logrolling offers. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15(2),
101–124. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.405
Movius,
H., & Wilson, T. D. (2011). How We Feel about the Deal. Negotiation
Journal, 27(2), 241–250. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1571-9979.2011.00305.x
Patton, B.
(2015). Managing the Value and Danger of Dualities. Negotiation Journal,
31(4), 349–352. https://doi.org/10.1111/nejo.12104
Sinaceur,
M., & Neale, M. A. (2005). Not All Threats are Created Equal: How
Implicitness and Timing Affect the Effectiveness of Threats in Negotiations. Group
Decision and Negotiation, 14(1), 63–85. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10726-005-3876-5
Steinel,
W., Van Kleef, G. A., & Harinck, F. (2008). Are you talking to me?!
Separating the people from the problem when expressing emotions in negotiation.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(2), 362–369. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2006.12.002
Wang, J.,
& Zionts, S. (2005). Negotiating Wisely: Considerations Based on MCDM/MAUT.
SSRN Electronic Journal. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.790944
Weinstein,
N., Itzchakov, G., & Legate, N. (2022). The motivational value of listening
during intimate and difficult conversations. Social and Personality
Psychology Compass, 16(2). https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12651
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.