Group Members:
Alyssa
Rashid, 1941356; Yiqin Cai,
1666921
Eric Lin,
1717147; Glenn Song,
1734280
Snooze, Don't Lose!
Feeling
Sleepy?
Ever find yourself yawning in class? Or
even falling asleep during a lecture? You aren’t alone! Insufficient
sleep is an issue for a majority of university students. More than 25% of
students reported having less than 6.5 hours of sleep per night (H. G. Lund et
al., 2010). The average bedtime for students is around 12:30-1:30 am, being
even later on weekends (Tsai, L. L., & Li, S. P, 2004). More than one third
of university students claimed they stay up until 3 am at least once a week (H.
G. Lund et al., 2010), indicating that irregular sleep patterns are common
amongst college students.
Although sufficient scientific
literature is lacking, our experience as university students tells us that bedtime
procrastination is a common phenomenon for many people around
us(Kadzikowska-Wrzosek, 2018). Bedtime procrastination, is defined by
Kroese (2014) as the intentional delay of going to sleep without any external
restrictions. It describes the tendency to put off sleep every night to watch
that extra episode (or several), mindlessly scrolling through Instagram, etc.
People who procrastinate before bedtime have lower self-regulation compared to
those who do not (Kroese, 2014). Therefore, our project aims to teach our
audience some behaviour change techniques and strategies to help better
regulate themselves and say goodbye to bedtime procrastination.
Why Should I Care? The Significance of
the project
“Early to bed and early
to rise, makes a man healthy, wealthy and wise.” - Benjamin Franklin.
Getting into bed early has countless
physical and mental health benefits.
Productivity
First,
going to sleep even just an hour earlier can lead to increased productivity the
next day, whereas drowsiness and fatigue causes a decrease in logic, reasoning,
memory and concentration leading to a major decline in productivity. A study
conducted by Diekelmann & Born (2010) found that participants performed
better at a puzzle task after having had quality sleep compared to those who
were disrupted in their slumber. They were also better at solving complex
problems, leading researchers to conclude that sleeping earlier greatly
increases productivity. Productivity has always been a crucial element for
university students, because their learning outcomes and grades are largely
determined by their productivity. Therefore, students should not underestimate
the importance of sleeping early.
Physical
health
In
terms of physical health, sleeping early promotes weight loss, subsequently
reducing the risk of obesity and heart disease. Plus - who doesn’t want to lose weight?
Several studies have examined the relation between sleep and weight and have
shown that individuals who sleep later are 1.5 times more likely to be obese
than those who go to bed early. (Olds, Maher & Matricciani, 2011) This is
because our bodies crave more junk food in the later hours due to a hormone
called leptin which is secreted to alter food intake and regulate energy
expenditure. It has also been noted that there is a positive correlation
between bad sleep quality and severe physiological diseases. The evidence
collected in a large-scale study suggests that shortened or disrupted sleep is
a predictor of many critical illnesses such as certain types of cancer (Lockley
& Foster, 2012).
Mental
health
Ever
find yourself getting annoyed by your coworkers? Irritated by your friends and
family? Sleeping early and getting good quality sleep increases mood and leads
to a more positive state of mind. A study by Nota & Coles (2014)
investigated the association between RNT (repeated negative thinking) and sleep
duration. Results demonstrated that individuals who sleep late and experience
disrupted sleep are more likely to have negative and pessimistic thoughts.
Authors also commented that creating a healthy sleep schedule and getting
enough rest leads to better emotion regulation. Now you’ll be able to avoid
those pesky arguments with your loved ones!
Our
project also has educational significance for our audience. We taught our
audience one of the most effective self-regulatory
strategies: implementation intention (which will be mentioned
later), that could benefit them for the rest of their life. Although in our
project, implementation intention was only used to establish good sleep habits,
this self-regulatory strategy can nevertheless be applied to achieve countless
other goals by creating habits. Guiding them how to use implementation
intention to form good sleep habits can familiarize them with this strategy and
how it can be used for various other purposes. Therefore, our project can serve
as a good starting point for our audience to understand and grasp the core of
this important self-regulatory strategy.
Our
Intervention
We
decided to help university students with bedtime procrastination, to become
good sleepers and develop good sleeping habits. A good sleep routine consists
of two elements: first going to bed early and second, getting enough sleep. The
key is to figure out a sleep schedule that people can stick with every single
night — even on weekends.
Staying up late on the weekends can make it difficult to get back on track
during the week. Therefore, if this schedule is to be kept in the long run
(hopefully, for the rest of one’s life), it should be something that is
simple and automatic, requiring no conscious effort. One way is to set
implementation intentions, as previously mentioned. This is because, by doing
so, actions are gradually integrated into a routine, eventually making the task
automatic and turning the action into a habit.
We
created two posters. The first one was to catch people’s attention and
introduce the aim of our project. This was posted on our individual social
medias, as well as handed out to our friends and flatmates. The second poster
contained the specific instructions to be followed in order to establish a good
sleep routine. The instruction poster was not posted on social media and was
only given to students who showed interest in participating in our project.
In
our first poster, we illustrate the effectiveness of our methods, and provided
our email address for those who are interested to contact us.
In
our instruction poster, we gave our audience step-by-step instructions to teach
them how to set an implementation intention plan to form a sleep habit.
Implementation
Intention
People
generally have positive
intentions, but can often fail to act on them (Orbell, 1998).
Implementation intention is often used to avoid this situation. This is a
self-regulatory strategy used to achieve goals in which people need to specify
a behaviour that they will perform and a situational context (Gollwitzer,
2006). This strategy is usually presented in the form of an ‘if-then’ plan. For example: “if situation Y is
encountered, then I will initiate goal-directed behaviour X”. While goal
intentions are more vague and abstract, implementation intentions are much more
concrete and procedural. The automatization of the behaviour in response to a
situational cue can remove all hesitation and deliberation on the part of the
decision maker when such a situation arises, and it can shield people from
goal-threatening distractions.
We
also suggested to our participants to go to bed between 8pm to 12pm
because the National Sleep Foundation indicates that it is best to fall asleep
sometime within this time frame (Kristeen, 2019). We also offered some general
tips to make it easier for our audience to form good sleep habits (see Fig.
instruction poster).
In
our project, for example, a participant wanted to form a good habit of sleeping
from 11pm to 7am everyday. Through learning from our instruction poster, the
participant learned to set ‘if-then’ plans as follows:
‘If it is 11pm, then I
will put my phone into the drawer and be in bed.’
‘If the alarm goes off
in the morning (at 7am), then I will go to my bathroom and wash my face’.
In
the end, this participant would go to bed as soon as he sees that the time is
11 o’clock while ignoring
all distractions and temptations.
How
our intervention was tailored for our audience
Our
project specifically targeted students as they are one of the most sleep deprived
age groups due to stress from school, technology and poor time management. The
Internet is a useful tool to spread information, especially targeting young
adults. Young adults trust online information and consider the Internet as a
valid source of health advice (Beck, 2014). Since they have the tendency to
seek health information on social media (Zheng, 2014), we put our posters on
various social media namely Instagram, Wechat, Whatsapp and Facebook to reach
the maximum number of individuals possible. Due to the busy lifestyle, students
may not always pay close attention to messages posted online, and may exhibit
information skimming behaviour (Khoo, 2014).Therefore, we designed a poster
that is used specifically for attracting our students' attention. In addition,
we handed out the poster to our friends and flatmates as they are easily
accessible and were more willing to participate in our project. We each handed
a poster to a minimum of 10 friends and flatmates. The poster would have been
seen by approximately 300-400 people on the Internet judging by the number of
views it received, the amount of times it was shared, and the number of
followers each of us have. In the end, we managed to get 15 individuals to
participate in the project to date.
Persuasion
techniques
Audience’s attention
The
first step to effective persuasion is to grab attention. Creating curiosity can
help us to do that. People become curious when there is a gap between what they
want to know and what they currently know. People would give their attention
when others have information they are lacking (Schmitt, 2008; Gottlieb et al.,
2013). Our subtitle ‘things a good sleeper will never tell
you’ could induce
curiosity in our audience, and thus getting our audience interested in what we
have to tell.
Our
poster could be one of the dozens of things they see in a day, therefore people
could easily lose interest. In order to keep attention, instead of crowding all
the information onto a single page, we put the instructions section onto another
poster. By doing this, our audience’s attention is more likely going to stay
focused on the first poster and therefore, would be more willing to carefully
learn our methods later.
Commitment
Our
implementation intention helps people form an action plan which then turns into
a commitment. Since breaking a commitment would cause discomfort (Cialdini,
1984), our audience would be more likely to follow their plans than breaking
them. In addition, by making a commitment publicly known, one is more likely to
stick with the commitment and plan (Cialdini, 1998). For example, it was found
that people who made a public commitment of recycling did more recycling than
those who did not make the commitment (Burn, 1986). Inspired by these studies,
we encouraged our participants to join our group chat on Whatsapp where our
participants shared their own experience on forming a good sleeping
habit.
Yale
Approach
Yale's
approach suggests that people are more likely to believe information from a
reliable source than a less credible source (Aronson, 2019). For example,
Hovland and Weiss (1951) found that people are more likely to be persuaded by a
prestigious journal than by an ordinary one. We made it clear that the efficacy
of our behaviour change strategies were proven by a large number of studies
conducted by experts. We also presented to our audience the quote of Thomas
Hills, a Psychology professor of University of Warwick, “It (implementation
intention) is one of the most important strategies that should be learned about
behaviour change”. This is useful because a professor
from a prestigious university is recognised as an authority figure, thus having
effect on people’s judgement (Matthew, 2009). In
addition, people are more willing to accept and be persuaded by those who are
similar to them (Baskett, 1973). Students are more likely to relate to our
posters because we all share the same identity as university students.
Pressure
inducing strategies and Effort heuristic
We
market our behaviour change strategies as an limited-time offer, because we do
not want to give the impression that our strategy is a free gift. Studies have
shown that people tend to discount the value of something if it was given to
them for free (Park, 2019; Priya, 2004). By presenting our behaviour change
strategies in this way, we attempted to prevent people from discounting their
value. What is more, limited offers can also induce pressure thus increasing
people’s tendency to take
action (Wearn, 2018). In addition, the poster advertised that only fifty people
can get access to our methods. Limiting the number of offers could make people
feel like they are competing with others and give them the impression that they
are missing out if they hesitate. These pressure inducing strategies aimed to
make people more likely to reach back to us proactively (i.e. emailing us to
get access to our strategies). According to the theory of effort heuristic,
people tend to allocate a higher value to things that they made more effort
for, than those obtained with less effort (Kruger, 2004). Therefore, those who
reached back to us through email —— which is seen as an effort made on the
part of our audience —— would be more likely to treat our
strategies seriously and follow our instructions.
How
behaviour changes can be measured
In a meta-analysis examining the
benefits of monitoring goal progress, researchers found that people who engage
in progress monitoring are more likely to stick to their plans (Harkin, 2016).
This motivates us to encourage our audience to monitor themselves. Doing so has
double significance, first, it gives us a chance to measure the extent of
effectiveness of our intervention; second, it allows our audience to be
consistent in maintaining good sleeping habits.
A sleep diary is a very popular tool
used in research to measure sleep behaviour (Monk, 1994). In order to measure
the sleep behaviour of our participants. We also used an extremely simplified
version of sleep diary (Carney et al., 2012). The diary was designed to gather
information about our participants’ sleep. Since our intervention is
centred around bedtime procrastination, we focused on the two following aspects
of our participants’ sleep:
1. What time do they go to bed without
procrastinating (e.g. watching videos)?
2. What time do they get out of the bed
in the morning?
We suggest to our audience they can
record their sleep in three ways, record it in a notebook, record it on a
phone or record it on Google Calendar. Google Calendar is very straightforward
and one can easily see the trend of their sleep behaviour. We leave the
question of which one to choose to our audience themselves as it is a matter of
personal preference.
Future
improvements
In
addition to spreading information via social media, we can take more offline
actions such as setting up posters in front of the library and other places
where many students would pass by to attract more attention. A study identified
that face-to-face communication can facilitate teaching, learning, motivation
and persuasion (Kumar,
L., 2013). Therefore,
we should interact more with students face-to-face in the future. Interactive
video learning can produce higher learning effectiveness satisfaction than
traditional ways of teaching (Zhang, D. et al., 2006). Thus, we should also attempt
to improve our teaching quality and efficiency by creating a video or
animation.
That
being said: Snooze, don't Lose!
References
Aronson,
E., Wilson, T. D., & Sommers, S. (2019). Social psychology. New York,
NY: Pearson.
Baskett,
G. D. (1973). Interview decisions are determined by competency and
attitude similarity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 57(3), 343.
Beck,
F., Richard, J., Nguyen-Thanh, V., Montagni, I., Parizot, I., & Renahy, E.
(2014). Use of the Internet as a Health Information Resource Among French Young
Adults: Results From a Nationally Representative Survey. Journal Of Medical
Internet Research, 16(5), e128. doi: 10.2196/jmir.2934
Burn,
S. M. , & Oskamp, S. (1986). Increasing community recycling with persuasive
communication and public commitment. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,
16, 29-41.
Carney, C. E., Buysse, D. J.,
Ancoli-Israel, S., Edinger, J. D., Krystal, A. D., Lichstein, K. L., &
Morin, C. M. (2012). The consensus sleep diary: standardizing prospective sleep
self-monitoring. Sleep, 35(2), 287-302.
Cialdini,
R. B. (1984). Chapter 3: Commitment and consistency: hobgoblins of the mind.
Influence: The psychology of persuasion. New York, NY: Harper Collins
Publishing.
Cialdini,
R. B., & Trost, M. R. (1998). Social Influence: social norms, conformity,
and compliance. In The Handbook of Social Psychology, 4th ed.
Gilbert, D. T., Fiske, S.T., & Lindzey, G. Vol. 2, 151–92.
Diekelmann,
S., & Born, J. (2010, January 4). The memory function of sleep. Retrieved
from https://www.nature.com/articles/nrn2762
Gollwitzer,
P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and goal
achievement: A meta‐analysis of effects and processes. Advances
in experimental social psychology, 38, 69-119.
Harkin,
B., Webb, T. L., Chang, B. P. I., Prestwich, A., Conner, M., Kellar, I., Benn,
Y., & Sheeran, P. (2016). Does monitoring goal progress promote goal
attainment? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychological
Bulletin, 142(2), 198–229.
Hovland,
C. I., & Weiss, W. (1951). The Influence of Source Credibility on
Communication Effectiveness. Public Opinion Quarterly,15(4), 635.
Gottlieb,
J., Oudeyer, P., Lopes, M., & Baranes, A. (2013). Information-seeking,
curiosity, and attention: computational and neural mechanisms. Trends In
Cognitive Sciences, 17(11), 585-593. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.001
Kadzikowska-Wrzosek,
R. (2018a). Self-regulation and bedtime procrastination: The role of
self-regulation skills and chronotype. Personality and Individual
Differences, 128,10–15.
Khoo,
C.S.G.: Issues in Information Behavior on Social Media. Libres 24(2), 75–96 (2014)
Kroese,
FM, de Ridder, DTD, Evers, C. (2014) Bedtime procrastination: Introducing a new
area of procrastination. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 611.
Kristeen
Cherney (2019), Best Time to Sleep and Wake Up, Healthline. https://www.healthline.com/health/best-time-to-sleep
Kruger,
J., Wirtz, D., Boven, L., & Altermatt, T. (2004). The effort heuristic. Journal
of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 91-98.
Kumar,
L. (2013). Effective Communication for Teaching and Learning. A book
published by Patna Women's College, Patna–Educational Technology
in Teaching and Learning Prospects and Challenges, 83-90.
Lee
Kavanau, J. (2005). Evolutionary approaches to understanding sleep. Sleep
Medicine Reviews, 9(2), 141-152. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2004.11.002
Matthew Inglis & Juan Pablo
Mejia-Ramos (2009) The Effect of Authority on the Persuasiveness of
Mathematical Arguments, Cognition and Instruction, 27:1, 25-50
Monk
TH, Reynolds CF, Kupfer DJ, Buysse DJ, Coble PA, Hayes AJ, Machen MA, Petrie
SR, Ritenour AM. The Pittsburgh Sleep Diary. J Sleep Res. 1994;3:111-120
Nota, J. A., & Coles, M. E. (2014).
Duration and Timing of Sleep are Associated with Repetitive Negative Thinking. Cognitive
Therapy and Research, 39(2), 253–261. doi:
10.1007/s10608-014-9651-7
Olds, T., Maher, C., & Matricciani,
L. (2011). Sleep Duration or Bedtime? Exploring the Relationship between Sleep
Habits and Weight Status and Activity Patterns. Sleep, 34(10),
1299-1307. doi: 10.5665/sleep.1266
Orbell,S., & Sheeran, P. (1998)
Regulation of behaviour in pursuit of health goals: Commentary. Psychology
and Health, 13, 753-758
Raghubir,
P. (2004). Free Gift with Purchase: Promoting or Discounting the Brand?. Journal
Of Consumer Psychology, 14(1-2), 181-186. doi:
10.1207/s15327663jcp1401&2_20
Santos,
M. D., Leve, C., & Pratkanis, A. R. (1994). Hey Buddy, Can You Spare
Seventeen Cents? Mindful Persuasion and the Pique Technique 1. Journal of
Applied Social Psychology, 24(9), 755-764.c
Schmitt, F. F., & Lahroodi, R.
(2008). The epistemic value of curiosity. Educational Theory, 58, 125–148.
Tsai, L. L., & Li, S. P. (2004).
Sleep patterns in college students: Gender and grade differences. Journal of
psychosomatic research, 56(2), 231-237
Wearn,
R. (2018). Warning on 'misleading' Black Friday deals. BBC News. [ONLINE]
Available at: https://www.bbc.co.uk/news/business-46301823
Y
Park, Y Yi. (2019). When free gifts hurt the promoted product: The influence of
product -gift fit on activating persuasion knowledge and devaluating the
promoted product. European Journal of Marketing.
Zhang, D., Zhou, L., Briggs, R. O.,
& Nunamaker Jr, J. F. (2006). Instructional video in e-learning: Assessing
the impact of interactive video on learning effectiveness. Information &
management,
43(1), 15-27.
Zheng, Y. (2014). Patterns
and motivations of young adults’ health information acquisitions on Facebook. Journal of Consumer
Health on the Internet, 18(2),157-175.
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.