In this advertisement, Peta, a large
international animal rights group are advising consumers to lose weight by
going vegetarian, whilst using one of their very famous and well known
arguments “save the whales”. They are using a fear appeal, suggesting that
larger people and non-vegetarians on the beach will be seen as fat whales,
giving vegetarianism as the solution. This may not be effective as it insults
its target audience, and only gives a vague solution to their fear appeal
problem. Many people may not know where to start with vegetarianism, and the ad
provides no source where they can find further information. Messages without
specific, doable recommendations tend to make the consumer avoid the issue
completely, demonstrating this billboard may have had the opposite effect to
what it intended, (Maddux & Rogers, 1983).
A more effective solution would have been
to use a just plain folks: similarity altercast method. This method reinforces
how similar the audience is to the person in the ad, informing them that their
behaviours and opinions can easily to change to the target one (in this case,
vegetarianism). For example, the ad could have shown an average “all American”
family, looking healthy and happy, with a tagline suggesting they were once
larger and unhealthier but they went vegetarian and lost the blubber. This
would reach a wide audience of consumers, who would see the billboard as a
realistic reminder of how they could help their family to be healthier. It is
important that the family on the billboard are ‘just plain folks’, so that
similarities may be struck between them and a wide range of members of the
public. E.g. they could be a family of 4 with a dog, making all members of
families of 4 feel similar, and likely to sway to their opinion, or all dog
owners feel similar. This has been effective in many studies, with even
seemingly irrelevant similarities influencing opinions, for example, music
preference.
In a study by Stotland,
Zander and Natsoulas (1964), female participants were taken into a
cubicle and given microphones through which to speak to the experimenter, and
other participants. In reality these microphones were dead, and they were
listening to a pre-recorded transcript. The participant was told that the other
people she could hear were person A and B and she was C and that after each
question she was to write down their responses and her own. First though, they
all listened to various melodies and stated their favourites, and the
participant was made aware that participant A or B happened to like most of the
same ones as her, making them similar. The participants then stated which
nonsense syllable out of a set they most preferred, out of a set of two shown
to them on cards. The participant previously recorded A and B’s preferences
(the first syllable or the second syllable) before choosing her own. She was
led to believe her microphone broke, meaning the others could not hear her
response. The participant then responded to a questionnaire about her
preference of names, in which she was also asked to say what she thought A and
B’s preferences would be.
As the table above shows, generalization of interpersonal similarity was highest with people who strongly preferred one of the songs, and they were more likely to be aware of their similarity with the confederate who showed the same preferences. 37 of those with strong musical preferences rated themselves as most similar to the confederate with the same preferences as themselves, vs. only 11 who did the same with weaker musical preferences. Therefore, it was found that participants noticed similarities between themselves and the other participant, and tended to
agree with the person they perceived as musically similar to themselves, choosing
the same syllables as them, provided that her opinions on the music were strong.
Participants also projected their preferences onto the person they perceived as
being similar to them, stating that they would prefer the same names they did.
This demonstrates that, for example, the Peta
billboard could instead have shown a family of four (the most common family
number, therefore the most similar to a wide range of people) supporting going
vegetarian, as it would make people think this was a choice they could easily
make as well.
References
Maddux, J. E., & Rogers, R. W. (1983). Protection motivation and self-efficacy: A revised theory of fear appeals and attitude change. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19(5), 469-479.
Stotland, E.,
Zander, A., & Natsoulas, T. (1964). Generalization of interpersonal
similarity. The
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62(2), 250-256.
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.